Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 11 de 11
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis ; 31(4): A148-58, 2014 Apr 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24695163

ABSTRACT

Foveal and peripheral hue-scaling data were obtained for a 1° foveal stimulus and a 3° stimulus presented at 10° retinal eccentricity under both bleach (reducing rod input) and no-bleach (permitting rod input) conditions. Uniform appearance diagrams (UADs) were generated from the data. Peripheral stimuli appeared more saturated than foveal stimuli (i.e., supersaturated), especially in the green-yellow region of the UADs. This effect was particularly pronounced for the peripheral bleach condition. The range of wavelengths perceived as green-yellow in the peripheral retina was expanded as compared to the fovea, while the range of wavelengths experienced as blue-green was compressed. This indicates that there are shifts in the unique hue loci with retinal location. While several factors can be ruled out as potential causes for these perceptual differences, the underlying mechanism of this supersaturation effect in the peripheral retina is unknown.


Subject(s)
Color Perception , Retina/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Middle Aged
2.
J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis ; 29(2): A44-51, 2012 Feb 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22330404

ABSTRACT

Hue-scaling data were collected from three observers using the "4+1" color-naming procedure for circular (0.25°-5°), monochromatic (440-660 nm) stimuli. Stimuli were presented at ±10° along the vertical and horizontal meridians under conditions chosen to include both rod and cone signals (no bleach) and to minimize rod contribution (bleach). All color-naming data were analyzed and compared using uniform appearance diagrams. Smaller stimuli appear more desaturated under both bleach conditions. This effect is particularly detrimental for the perception of green and is influenced by retinal location and exacerbated with rod input. As stimulus size increases and perceptive field sizes are filled for all four elemental hues, the differences in hue perception among the four peripheral locations and the two bleach conditions are attenuated. Results are consistent with predictions based on known differences in the underlying retinal mosaic among the four locations.


Subject(s)
Color Perception , Photic Stimulation/methods , Adult , Color , Female , Humans , Middle Aged , Retina/physiology , Time Factors
3.
J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis ; 28(12): 2600-6, 2011 Dec 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22193273

ABSTRACT

Hue-discrimination functions were derived from hue-naming data (480-620 nm, 20 nm steps) obtained in 4 min intervals from 4 min to 28 min postbleach at 10° temporal retinal eccentricity. Hue-naming data were also obtained in the fovea. Hue-discrimination functions derived at the 4, 8, and 12 min intervals were very similar to those derived in the fovea. As time postbleach exceeded 12 min and rod sensitivity increased, the shape of the hue-discrimination functions changed. Most notably, the minimum between 560-580 nm disappeared and the just noticeable differences (JNDs) for the longer wavelength stimuli increased. The long-wavelength suppression in hue discrimination may be due to rod input in the magnocellular pathway interacting and affecting the long-wavelength sensitivity of the parvocellular pathway.

4.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt ; 30(5): 545-52, 2010 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20883338

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Studies investigating the effect of rods on unique hue loci in the peripheral retina generally obtain measures at two time points associated with the dark adaptation function - the cone plateau and the rod plateau. In comparison, this study used a color-naming procedure to identify the loci of unique green and unique yellow as a function of time associated with the entire dark adaptation function. The unique hue loci derived by this procedure were then compared to those obtained directly with a staircase procedure. METHOD: Hue-scaling functions were obtained for monochromatic stimuli for four observers using the '4 + 1' procedure. Data were collected every 4 min following extinction of a bleaching stimulus. These hue-scaling functions were then converted to uniform appearance diagrams (UADs) to derive unique green and unique yellow loci. Unique green and unique yellow loci were also obtained from the same observers via a staircase procedure at 4-9 min post-bleach (minimal rod input) and after 28 min dark adaptation (maximal rod input). Measurements were made in the peripheral retina at 10° temporal retinal eccentricity and at the fovea. RESULTS: Unique green loci derived from UADs are at longer wavelengths compared to those measured directly with the staircase procedure. In addition, unique green loci derived from UADs show a progressive shift to longer wavelengths as time post-bleach increases; whereas, unique green loci obtained from the staircase procedure differ little between the rod-bleach and no-bleach conditions. Unique yellow loci are similar across both experimental procedures. CONCLUSION: Unique green loci derived from UADs are not the same as those measured with traditional psychophysical procedures. These differences may be due to the different response criteria used by observers in the color-naming and staircase procedures. The unique green loci obtained from UADs indicate that rod signals shift unique green loci to longer wavelengths as time post-bleach increases. More direct methods need to be employed to determine if this rod effect is valid.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Dark Adaptation/physiology , Female , Fovea Centralis/physiology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Photic Stimulation/methods , Psychophysics , Retinal Rod Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Young Adult
5.
Psychophysiology ; 46(4): 694-702, 2009 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19490514

ABSTRACT

Multistable perception occurs when a single physical stimulus leads to two or more distinct percepts that spontaneously switch (reverse). Previous ERP studies have reported reversal negativities and late positive components associated with perceptual reversals. The goal of the current study was to localize the neural generators of the reversal ERP components in order to evaluate their correspondence with previous fMRI results and to better understand their functional significance. A Necker-type stimulus was presented for brief intervals while subjects indicated their perceptions. Local auto-regressive average source analyses and dipole modeling indicated that sources for the reversal negativity were located in inferior occipital-temporal cortex. Generators of the late positive component were estimated to reside in inferior temporal and superior parietal regions.


Subject(s)
Electroencephalography , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Form Perception/physiology , Optical Illusions/physiology , Adaptation, Psychological/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Attention/physiology , Evoked Potentials, Visual/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Photic Stimulation , Young Adult
6.
J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis ; 26(5): 1167-77, 2009 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19412234

ABSTRACT

The different hemifields in the retina are known to vary in photoreceptor density as well as in the number of photoreceptors converging onto one ganglion cell. The effect of these differences among the retinal hemifields at 10 degrees retinal eccentricity was investigated using a color-naming procedure to derive perceptive field sizes for the hue terms of blue, green, yellow, and red. Color-naming data were obtained under two conditions: (1) after a bleach condition, chosen to minimize rod contribution, and (2) after 30 min dark adaptation, chosen to maximize rod contribution. Perceptive field sizes measured in the bleach condition were consistent with degree of neural convergence of cones to ganglion cells across the retina rather than differences in cone density. Rod densities relative to cone densities correlated with the size of perceptive fields in the no-bleach condition, i.e., the greater the rod:cone ratio, the larger the perceptive field.

7.
Brain Cogn ; 67(1): 11-24, 2008 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18155339

ABSTRACT

A longstanding debate exists in the literature concerning bottom-up vs. top-down influences on bistable perception. Recently, a technique has been developed to measure early changes in brain activity (via ERPs) related to perceptual reversals (Kornmeier & Bach, 2004). An ERP component, the reversal negativity (RN) has been identified, and is characterized as an increase in negative potential over the posterior scalp from 150 to 350 ms for perceptual reversals compared to perceptual stability. This finding, although interesting, has not helped resolve issues related to the bottom-up vs. top-down debate because top-down influences have not been directly manipulated. The current study focused on resolving some of these issues by measuring the RN while observers maintained one of three 'intentional approaches', (1) try to reverse perception as often as possible, (2) try to stabilize perception for as long as possible, and (3) maintain a passive approach. Enhancements in RN amplitude were found for the intention-to-reverse condition compared to the passive condition. This finding suggests an early influence (150 ms) of top-down control on perceptual reversals of bistable figures. Results are discussed in terms of competing attention shifting vs. fatigue-based theories of bistable perception.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Evoked Potentials, Visual , Form Perception/physiology , Intention , Adolescent , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Female , Humans , Male , Psychological Theory , Visual Perception/physiology
8.
J Vis ; 7(1): 6, 2007 Jan 22.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17461674

ABSTRACT

Electrophysiological recordings were made in 21 observers to investigate whether differences in signature components (P1, N1, selection negativity [SN]) would be revealed during perceptual reversals of three different multistable figures. Using a lattice of Necker cubes as a stimulus, J. Kornmeier and M. Bach (2004, 2005) reported differences in P1 amplitudes as well a broad reversal-related negativity occurring 200-400 ms poststimulus. The current study investigated whether these event-related potentials of Necker cube reversals represent general "perceptual switching" mechanisms and would, therefore, be common to other types of multistable figures. Three different types of multistable stimuli were utilized: a modified Rubin's face/vase, a modified Schröder's staircase, and a novel natural stimulus, Lemmo's cheetahs. Results revealed the broad reversal-related negativity for the face/vase and the reversible staircase but not for the cheetahs. This component is comparable to the SN in polarity, latency, and scalp topography. An effect of early visual spatial attention on figure reversals was suggested by an analysis of the occipital P1 and N1 components. The P1, N1, or both were enhanced for trials in which the observer reported perceptual reversals compared with trials in which no reversals were reported for the face/vase and reversible staircase stimuli. These results support a model of multistable perception in which changes in early spatial attention (indicated by P1 and N1 enhancement) modulate perceptual reversals (indicated by the reversal negativity or SN).


Subject(s)
Electroencephalography , Evoked Potentials, Visual , Photic Stimulation/methods , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Attention/physiology , Brain Mapping , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Occipital Lobe/physiology , Parietal Lobe/physiology , Reaction Time , Space Perception/physiology
9.
J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis ; 22(10): 2137-42, 2005 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16277283

ABSTRACT

The effects of intensity on chromatic perceptive field size were investigated along the horizontal meridian at 10 degrees temporal eccentricity by manipulating stimulus intensity from 0.3 to 3.3 log trolands. Following light adaptation, observers described the hue and saturation of monochromatic stimuli (440-660 nm, in 10 nm steps) for a series of test sizes (0.098-3 degrees) presented along the time period associated with the cone plateau of the dark-adaptation function. Perceptive field sizes of the four elemental hues (red, green, yellow, and blue) and the saturation component were estimated by three observers at each intensity level for each wavelength. In general, perceptive field sizes of blue and red are the smallest, and yellow and green are the largest. Furthermore, perceptive field sizes of all four hues decrease with increasing stimulus intensity, though the absolute change is largest for green and yellow. The decrease in size with increase in intensity cannot be completely explained in terms of saturation or rod signals and is likely, then, attributable to a cone-based mechanism.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Ocular/physiology , Color Perception/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Visual Fields/physiology , Adaptation, Ocular/radiation effects , Adult , Color Perception/radiation effects , Contrast Sensitivity/radiation effects , Discrimination Learning/physiology , Discrimination Learning/radiation effects , Dose-Response Relationship, Radiation , Female , Humans , Light , Male , Radiation Dosage , Sensory Thresholds/radiation effects , Visual Fields/radiation effects
10.
J Vis ; 5(5): 435-43, 2005 May 18.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16097874

ABSTRACT

The effect of retinal illuminance (0.3-3.3 log td) on chromatic perceptive field size was investigated at 10 degrees eccentricity along the horizontal meridian of the temporal retina. Using the 4+1 color-naming procedure, observers described the hue and saturation of a series of monochromatic stimuli (440-660 nm, in 10-nm steps) of various test sizes (.098-5 degrees) after 30-min dark adaptation. Perceptive field sizes of the four elemental hues and the saturation component were estimated for each wavelength at each retinal illuminance. Results indicate that perceptive field sizes for blue, green, yellow, and saturation all decrease with increasing retinal illuminance; the perceptive field size for red is the smallest and invariant with intensity. The influence of rods on perceptive field size may account for some of the results; other factors are also considered.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Retina/radiation effects , Size Perception/physiology , Visual Fields/physiology , Adult , Contrast Sensitivity , Dark Adaptation , Female , Humans , Lighting , Male , Middle Aged
11.
Vision Res ; 44(16): 1891-906, 2004.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15145683

ABSTRACT

Experiments were conducted with a bipartite field to better understand the Bezold-Brücke hue shift in the peripheral retina. The first experiment measured hue shift in the fovea and at 1 degrees and 8 degrees along the horizontal meridian of the nasal retina for nominal test wavelengths of 430, 450, 490, 520 and 610 nm. Peripheral measurements were obtained under two adaptation conditions: after 30 min dark adaptation and following a rod-bleach. Results indicated that foveal hue shifts differed from those obtained after a rod-bleach. Data from the rod-bleach and no-bleach conditions in the periphery were similar, indicating that rods could not account for the differences between the foveal data and the rod-bleach peripheral data. Hue shifts obtained for the 520 nm test stimulus, and to a smaller extent other test wavelengths, at 8 degrees nasal retinal eccentricity revealed that the wavelength of the matching stimulus depended upon the lateral position of the matching and test fields, and this effect was greater in the no-bleach condition than the rod-bleach condition. Several factors were investigated in experiments 2 and 3 to explain the results with the 520 nm test field. It appears that differential rod density under the two half fields and the compression of photoreceptors by the optic disk may partially, but not fully, account for the 520 nm effect.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Retina/physiology , Dark Adaptation/physiology , Female , Fovea Centralis/physiology , Humans , Male , Optic Disk/physiology , Orientation/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Retinal Rod Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Visual Fields/physiology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...