Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 29
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 77(2): 278-286, 2024 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36891822

ABSTRACT

Pseudowords are letter strings that look like words but are not words. They are used in psycholinguistic research, particularly in tasks such as lexical decision. In this context, it is essential that the pseudowords respect the orthographic statistics of the target language. Pseudowords that violate them would be too easy to reject in a lexical decision and would not enforce word recognition on real words. We propose a new pseudoword generator, UniPseudo, using an algorithm based on Markov chains of orthographic n-grams. It generates pseudowords from a customizable database, which allows one to control the characteristics of the items. It can produce pseudowords in any language, in orthographic or phonological form. It is possible to generate pseudowords with specific characteristics, such as frequency of letters, bigrams, trigrams, or quadrigrams, number of syllables, frequency of biphones, and number of morphemes. Thus, from a list of words composed of verbs, nouns, adjectives, or adverbs, UniPseudo can create pseudowords resembling verbs, nouns, adjectives, or adverbs in any language using an alphabetic or syllabic system.


Subject(s)
Language , Reading , Humans , Psycholinguistics , Linguistics
2.
Appl Psychol Health Well Being ; 15(3): 846-864, 2023 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36320165

ABSTRACT

Low self-esteem is a vulnerability factor for depressive disorders, and the prevention of psychological disorders is essential in cancer patients. The enhancement of self-esteem in breast cancer patients may therefore be an appropriate clinical target. Previous studies have shown the efficacy of the Lexical Association Technique to enhance self-esteem in healthy subjects. This study aims to test the clinical efficacy and acceptability of the Lexical Association Technique on the self-esteem of cancer patients. A double-blind randomized controlled trial was conducted on 63 breast cancer patients during their radiotherapy treatment. Global self-esteem measures were taken using the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale before and after the Lexical Association Technique and 1 month after its end. The results showed a significant improvement in global self-esteem in patients immediately after performing the Lexical Association Technique compared to an active control group. However, the positive effects did not last 1 month. These results confirm the efficacy and suitability of the Lexical Association Technique for cancer patients. Avenues of research are proposed to extend the effects of the technique and increase its transdiagnostic applicability.


Subject(s)
Breast Neoplasms , Mental Disorders , Humans , Female , Self Concept , Treatment Outcome , Surveys and Questionnaires
3.
Front Psychol ; 12: 763900, 2021.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34777169

ABSTRACT

Numerous studies showed that cancer significantly increases the risk of developing depressive and anxious symptoms. It has been shown that self-esteem is an important psychological resource and is associated with many health behaviors. Furthermore, the vulnerability model of low self-esteem, which has received strong empirical support, highlights that low self-esteem is a real risk factor in the development of depressive disorders. This article aims at providing an overview of the involvement of self-esteem in the psychological adjustment to cancer. After briefly reviewing the literature, we suggest that its implication in the development of depressive disorders and its association with coping strategies and social support in cancer patients justify the consideration of self-esteem in oncology psychological care, especially in young adult patients and those with significant physical impairment following treatment.

4.
Behav Res Methods ; 52(6): 2480-2488, 2020 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32441034

ABSTRACT

Psycholinguistic research has shown that both the regularity and consistency of the grapheme-phoneme and phoneme-grapheme correspondences impact word processing. Lexique-Infra is a new database providing infra-lexical statistics for 137,717 French words. The frequencies of the grapheme-phoneme and phoneme-grapheme correspondences as well as other indicators (consistency, regularity, letter frequencies, bigrams, trigrams, phonemes, biphones, and syllables, etc.) are proposed and have been computed from the corpus of subtitles in Lexique 3.83. The aim of this new database is to propose numerous infra-lexical variables based on adult frequencies for a large number of words.


Subject(s)
Phonetics , Reading , Adult , Humans , Psycholinguistics
5.
Acta Psychol (Amst) ; 202: 102958, 2020 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31864215

ABSTRACT

A novel illusion entitled "the letter height superiority effect" has been demonstrated. This shows that letters are perceived as being taller than pseudoletters, while in reality their objective sizes are identical. An explanation of this illusion has been proposed in the framework of the Interactive Activation Model. Indeed, we postulated that the more a feature is activated, the taller a stimulus is perceived as being. The objective of the current study was to test this postulate by manipulating feature activation through signal-to-noise ratio. We presented gray stimuli (low signal-to-noise ratio) or black ones (high signal-to-noise ratio). In a first experiment, participants judged the size of pairs of either letters or pseudoletters presented as black or gray. In a second experiment we presented pairs consisting of a letter and a pseudoletter, of identical or different colors. In a third experiment, we presented pairs of letters or pseudoletters of identical or different colors by block to test the possible effect of previous exposure on perceptual judgments. The results showed that for identical objective size, participants perceive black stimuli to be taller than gray ones and that the effects of the nature of the stimuli and their color are cumulative. The results also indicated that the effects were not due to previous exposure to color or sizes. These results confirm the Interactive Activation Model as a credible explanation for the letter height superiority effect.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Illusions/physiology , Illusions/psychology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Judgment/physiology , Male , Reaction Time/physiology , Young Adult
6.
Cortex ; 116: 55-73, 2019 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30878180

ABSTRACT

The time-course of morphological processing during spoken word recognition was investigated using event-related brain potentials (ERPs) in an auditory lexical decision task. We compared three different types of French words: truly suffixed (e.g., pochette 'little pocket' = poche 'pocket' + diminutive suffix -ette), pseudo-suffixed (e.g., mouette 'seagull' = mou 'soft' + pseudo-suffix -ette) and non-suffixed target words (e.g., fortune 'fortune' = fort 'strong' + non-suffix -une). Suffixed (e.g., mouesse = mou + suffix -esse) and non-suffixed nonwords (e.g., mouipe = mou + non-suffix -ipe) were also tested. The behavioural results showed that participants responded more slowly to non-suffixed words than to truly suffixed and pseudo-suffixed words, but there was no difference between the two suffixed conditions. Moreover, participants made more errors rejecting pseudo-suffixed nonwords than non-suffixed nonwords. In the ERP analyses, T0 was shifted to the end of the embedded stem or pseudo-stem. The ERP results revealed enhanced N400 amplitudes for non-suffixed words compared to truly suffixed and pseudo-suffixed words. Again, there was no difference between the truly and pseudo-suffixed conditions. In addition, we found an increased N400 amplitude for both pseudo-suffixed and non-suffixed nonwords than for words. The latency of the onset of this N400 effect varied between the three experimental conditions: the word-nonword difference occurred earliest in the truly suffixed condition, slightly later in the pseudo-suffixed condition and latest in the non-suffixed condition. Both behavioural and EEG data jointly suggest that spoken words with a genuine morphological structure and words with a pseudo-morphological structure are decomposed into morphemic sub-units. Moreover, the earlier appearance of the N400 effects in the truly suffixed condition indicates that morphological information is more readily available in words with a semantically transparent morphological structure.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Decision Making/physiology , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Speech , Vocabulary , Adolescent , Adult , Electroencephalography/methods , Female , Humans , Male , Reaction Time , Reading , Semantics , Young Adult
7.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 71(4): 808-816, 2018 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28326995

ABSTRACT

Numerous studies in psychology, cognitive neuroscience and psycholinguistics have used pictures of objects as stimulus materials. Currently, authors engaged in cross-linguistic work or wishing to run parallel studies at multiple sites where different languages are spoken must rely on rather small sets of black-and-white or colored line drawings. These sets are increasingly experienced as being too limited. Therefore, we constructed a new set of 750 colored pictures of concrete concepts. This set, MultiPic, constitutes a new valuable tool for cognitive scientists investigating language, visual perception, memory and/or attention in monolingual or multilingual populations. Importantly, the MultiPic databank has been normed in six different European languages (British English, Spanish, French, Dutch, Italian and German). All stimuli and norms are freely available at http://www.bcbl.eu/databases/multipic .


Subject(s)
Language , Psycholinguistics , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Cluster Analysis , Culture , Europe , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation , Young Adult
8.
Behav Res Methods ; 50(3): 1285-1307, 2018 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28791657

ABSTRACT

Using the megastudy approach, we report a new database (MEGALEX) of visual and auditory lexical decision times and accuracy rates for tens of thousands of words. We collected visual lexical decision data for 28,466 French words and the same number of pseudowords, and auditory lexical decision data for 17,876 French words and the same number of pseudowords (synthesized tokens were used for the auditory modality). This constitutes the first large-scale database for auditory lexical decision, and the first database to enable a direct comparison of word recognition in different modalities. Different regression analyses were conducted to illustrate potential ways to exploit this megastudy database. First, we compared the proportions of variance accounted for by five word frequency measures. Second, we conducted item-level regression analyses to examine the relative importance of the lexical variables influencing performance in the different modalities (visual and auditory). Finally, we compared the similarities and differences between the two modalities. All data are freely available on our website ( https://sedufau.shinyapps.io/megalex/ ) and are searchable at www.lexique.org , inside the Open Lexique search engine.


Subject(s)
Databases, Factual , Decision Making , Language Arts , Search Engine , Data Accuracy , France , Humans , Reaction Time , Regression Analysis
9.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 166: 652-663, 2018 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29125951

ABSTRACT

How long does it take for word reading to become automatic? Does the appearance and development of automaticity differ as a function of orthographic depth (e.g., French vs. English)? These questions were addressed in a longitudinal study of English and French beginning readers. The study focused on automaticity as obligatory processing as measured in the Stroop test. Measures of decoding ability and the Stroop effect were taken at three time points during first grade (and during second grade in the United Kingdom) in 84 children. The study is the first to adjust the classic Stroop effect for inhibition (of distracting colors). The adjusted Stroop effect was zero in the absence of reading ability, and it was found to develop in tandem with decoding ability. After a further control for decoding, no effects of age or orthography were found on the adjusted Stroop measure. The results are in line with theories of the development of whole word recognition that emphasize the importance of the acquisition of the basic orthographic code.


Subject(s)
Aptitude , Automatism , Language , Phonetics , Reading , Stroop Test , Verbal Learning , Attention , Child , Color Perception , Female , Humans , Inhibition, Psychological , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Vocabulary
10.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 23(1): 291-8, 2016 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26370216

ABSTRACT

Letters are identified better when they are embedded within words rather than within pseudowords, a phenomenon known as the word superiority effect (Reicher in Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 275-280, 1969). This effect is, inter alia, accounted for by the interactive-activation model (McClelland & Rumelhart in Psychological Review, 88, 375-407, 1981) through feedback from word to letter nodes. In this study, we investigated whether overactivation of features could lead to perceptual bias, wherein letters would be perceived as being taller than pseudoletters, or words would be perceived as being taller than pseudowords. In two experiments, we investigated the effects of letter and lexical status on the perception of size. Participants who had to compare the heights of letters and pseudoletters, or of words and pseudowords, indeed perceived the former stimuli as being taller than the latter. Possible alternative interpretations of this height superiority effect for letters and words are discussed.


Subject(s)
Illusions/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Reading , Adult , Humans , Models, Psychological , Psycholinguistics
11.
Can J Exp Psychol ; 70(4): 316-324, 2016 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26640974

ABSTRACT

In this study, we explored the processing of singular and plural word forms, using megastudies in French, English, and Dutch. For singulars, we observed a base frequency effect but no surface frequency effect. For plurals, the effect depended on the frequency of the word form. When the word form had a frequency above a threshold value, we observed both surface and base frequency effects; for the frequencies below the threshold, we found a base frequency effect only, suggesting full decomposition for these words. The threshold differed between the languages, suggesting that more plurals are decomposed in French than in Dutch and more in Dutch than in English. In contrast, for all languages the singular form seems to be coactivated whenever the plural form is processed. These results are interpreted in light of some of the main models of morphological processing. (PsycINFO Database Record


Subject(s)
Language , Mathematics , Mental Processes/physiology , Semantics , Vocabulary , Humans , Regression Analysis
12.
Behav Res Methods ; 48(3): 963-72, 2016 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26170053

ABSTRACT

Lexical frequency is one of the strongest predictors of word processing time. The frequencies are often calculated from book-based corpora, or more recently from subtitle-based corpora. We present new frequencies based on Twitter, blog posts, or newspapers for 66 languages. We show that these frequencies predict lexical decision reaction times similar to the already existing frequencies, or even better than them. These new frequencies are freely available and may be downloaded from http://worldlex.lexique.org .


Subject(s)
Language , Psycholinguistics/statistics & numerical data , Social Media/statistics & numerical data , Blogging , Humans
13.
Behav Res Methods ; 45(3): 758-64, 2013 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23239073

ABSTRACT

The aim of this article is to describe a database of diphone positional frequencies in French. More specifically, we provide frequencies for word-initial, word-internal, and word-final diphones of all words extracted from a subtitle corpus of 50 million words that come from movie and TV series dialogue. We also provide intra- and intersyllable diphone frequencies, as well as interword diphone frequencies. To our knowledge, no other such tool is available to psycholinguists for the study of French sequential probabilities. This database and its new indicators should help researchers conducting new studies on speech segmentation.


Subject(s)
Databases, Factual , Language , Phonetics , Speech/classification , Adult , Humans , Psycholinguistics , Semantics
14.
Behav Res Methods ; 44(4): 991-7, 2012 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22396136

ABSTRACT

The SUBTLEX-US corpus has been parsed with the CLAWS tagger, so that researchers have information about the possible word classes (parts-of-speech, or PoSs) of the entries. Five new columns have been added to the SUBTLEX-US word frequency list: the dominant (most frequent) PoS for the entry, the frequency of the dominant PoS, the frequency of the dominant PoS relative to the entry's total frequency, all PoSs observed for the entry, and the respective frequencies of these PoSs. Because the current definition of lemma frequency does not seem to provide word recognition researchers with useful information (as illustrated by a comparison of the lemma frequencies and the word form frequencies from the Corpus of Contemporary American English), we have not provided a column with this variable. Instead, we hope that the full list of PoS frequencies will help researchers to collectively determine which combination of frequencies is the most informative.


Subject(s)
Algorithms , Language , Vocabulary , Humans , Psycholinguistics/methods , Terminology as Topic , Word Processing
15.
Front Psychol ; 2: 306, 2011.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22053160

ABSTRACT

We report performance measures for lexical decision (LD), word naming (NMG), and progressive demasking (PDM) for a large sample of monosyllabic monomorphemic French words (N = 1,482). We compare the tasks and also examine the impact of word length, word frequency, initial phoneme, orthographic and phonological distance to neighbors, age-of-acquisition, and subjective frequency. Our results show that objective word frequency is by far the most important variable to predict reaction times in LD. For word naming, it is the first phoneme. PDM was more influenced by a semantic variable (word imageability) than LD, but was also affected to a much greater extent by perceptual variables (word length, first phoneme/letters). This may reduce its usefulness as a psycholinguistic word recognition task.

16.
Acta Psychol (Amst) ; 138(2): 322-8, 2011 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21855049

ABSTRACT

In four experiments we examined whether the frequency of occurrence of letters affects performance in the alphabetic decision task (speeded letter vs. pseudo-letter classification). Experiments 1A and 1B tested isolated letters and pseudo-letters presented at fixation, and Experiments 2A and 2B tested the same stimuli inserted at the 1st, 3rd, or 5th position in a string of Xs. Significant negative correlations between letter frequency and response times to letter targets were found in all experiments. The correlations were found to be stronger for token frequency counts compared with both type frequency and frequency rank, stronger for frequency counts based on a book corpus compared with film subtitles, and stronger for measures counting occurrences as the first letter of words compared with inner letters and final letters. Correlations for letters presented in strings of Xs were found to depend on letter case and position-in-string. The results are in favor of models of word recognition that implement case-specific and position-specific letter representations.


Subject(s)
Decision Making/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Adult , Humans , Psychomotor Performance , Visual Perception/physiology , Vocabulary
17.
Front Psychol ; 2: 27, 2011.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21713191

ABSTRACT

In this Perspective Article we assess the usefulness of Google's new word frequencies for word recognition research (lexical decision and word naming). We find that, despite the massive corpus on which the Google estimates are based (131 billion words from books published in the United States alone), the Google American English frequencies explain 11% less of the variance in the lexical decision times from the English Lexicon Project (Balota et al., 2007) than the SUBTLEX-US word frequencies, based on a corpus of 51 million words from film and television subtitles. Further analyses indicate that word frequencies derived from recent books (published after 2000) are better predictors of word processing times than frequencies based on the full corpus, and that word frequencies based on fiction books predict word processing times better than word frequencies based on the full corpus. The most predictive word frequencies from Google still do not explain more of the variance in word recognition times of undergraduate students and old adults than the subtitle-based word frequencies.

18.
Behav Res Methods ; 42(3): 643-50, 2010 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20805586

ABSTRACT

We present a new database of Dutch word frequencies based on film and television subtitles, and we validate it with a lexical decision study involving 14,000 monosyllabic and disyllabic Dutch words. The new SUBTLEX frequencies explain up to 10% more variance in accuracies and reaction times (RTs) of the lexical decision task than the existing CELEX word frequency norms, which are based largely on edited texts. As is the case for English, an accessibility measure based on contextual diversity explains more of the variance in accuracy and RT than does the raw frequency of occurrence counts. The database is freely available for research purposes and may be downloaded from the authors' university site at http://crr.ugent.be/subtlex-nl or from http://brm.psychonomic-journals.org/content/supplemental.


Subject(s)
Language , Motion Pictures , Databases, Factual , Decision Making/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Netherlands , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Reproducibility of Results , Television , Young Adult
19.
Behav Res Methods ; 42(2): 488-96, 2010 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20479180

ABSTRACT

The French Lexicon Project involved the collection of lexical decision data for 38,840 French words and the same number of nonwords. It was directly inspired by the English Lexicon Project (Balota et al., 2007) and produced very comparable frequency and word length effects. The present article describes the methods used to collect the data, reports analyses on the word frequency and the word length effects, and describes the Excel files that make the data freely available for research purposes. The word and pseudoword data from this article may be downloaded from http://brm.psychonomic-journals.org/content/supplemental.


Subject(s)
Databases, Factual , Psycholinguistics/methods , Vocabulary , Adolescent , Adult , Decision Making , Female , France , Humans , Internet , Language , Male , Reaction Time , Software , Verbal Behavior
20.
Behav Res Methods ; 41(4): 977-90, 2009 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19897807

ABSTRACT

Word frequency is the most important variable in research on word processing and memory. Yet, the main criterion for selecting word frequency norms has been the availability of the measure, rather than its quality. As a result, much research is still based on the old Kucera and Francis frequency norms. By using the lexical decision times of recently published megastudies, we show how bad this measure is and what must be done to improve it. In particular, we investigated the size of the corpus, the language register on which the corpus is based, and the definition of the frequency measure. We observed that corpus size is of practical importance for small sizes (depending on the frequency of the word), but not for sizes above 16-30 million words. As for the language register, we found that frequencies based on television and film subtitles are better than frequencies based on written sources, certainly for the monosyllabic and bisyllabic words used in psycholinguistic research. Finally, we found that lemma frequencies are not superior to word form frequencies in English and that a measure of contextual diversity is better than a measure based on raw frequency of occurrence. Part of the superiority of the latter is due to the words that are frequently used as names. Assembling a new frequency norm on the basis of these considerations turned out to predict word processing times much better than did the existing norms (including Kucera & Francis and Celex). The new SUBTL frequency norms from the SUBTLEX(US) corpus are freely available for research purposes from http://brm.psychonomic-journals.org/content/supplemental, as well as from the University of Ghent and Lexique Web sites.


Subject(s)
Language , Vocabulary , Data Interpretation, Statistical , Humans , Memory/physiology , Mental Processes/physiology , Recognition, Psychology , Reference Standards
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...