Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 7 de 7
Filter
1.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31788009

ABSTRACT

Tu et al. (Emerg Themes Epidemiol 5:2, 2008. https://doi.org/10.1186/1742-7622-5-2) asserted that suppression, Simpson's Paradox, and Lord's Paradox are all the same phenomenon-the reversal paradox. In the reversal paradox, the association between an outcome variable and an explanatory (predictor) variable is reversed when another explanatory variable is added to the analysis. More specifically, Tu et al. (2008) purported to demonstrate that these three paradoxes are different manifestations of the same phenomenon, differently named depending on the scaling of the outcome variable, the explanatory variable, and the third variable. According to Tu et al. (2008), when all three variables are continuous, the phenomenon is called suppression; when all three variables are categorical, the phenomenon is called Simpson's Paradox; and when the outcome variable and the third variable are continuous but the explanatory variable is categorical, the phenomenon is called Lord's Paradox. We show that (a) the strong form of Simpson's Paradox is equivalent to negative suppression for a 2 × 2 × 2 contingency table, (b) the weak form of Simpson's Paradox is equivalent to classical suppression for a 2 × 2 × 2 contingency table, and (c) Lord's Paradox is not the same phenomenon as suppression or Simpson's Paradox.

2.
4.
Am Psychol ; 69(6): 626-8, 2014 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25197846

ABSTRACT

Comments on the article by Fredrickson and Losada (see record 2005-11834-001). Fredrickson (2013) herself, in reply to Brown et al. (2013), remarked that "most valuable to the maturation of this work will be longitudinal field studies and experiments that use densely repeated measures of emotions and relevant outcomes" (p. 820). This remark raises the question of why, if Fredrickson understands that a longitudinal (within-person across-time) study is needed to test the theory, this was not acknowledged in the initial article (Fredrickson & Losada, 2005), in the book that highlights the research of that article (Fredrickson, 2009, Chapter 7), or in the correction to the original article (Fredrickson & Losada, 2013), the latter of which insisted on the validity of the results of Fredrickson and Losada's (2005) within time across-persons study. Indeed, why wasn't the appropriate study design used in the first place? After all, it has been pointed out many times over many years that the data and the analysis used to test a theory should correspond to that theory and that a test of a within-person theory nearly always requires within-person data and analysis. Doing research the wrong way, while delaying doing it the right way "until later," is not acceptable after so many years of discussions of this issue.


Subject(s)
Affect , Mental Health , Models, Psychological , Female , Humans , Male
5.
Multivariate Behav Res ; 43(4): 556-63, 2008.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26771427

ABSTRACT

Paulhus, Robins, Trzesniewski, and Tracy (Multivariate Behavioral Research, 2004 , 39, 305-328) suggested that the three types of two-predictor suppression situations-classical suppression, cooperative suppression, and net suppression--can all be considered special cases of mutual suppression, in that the magnitude of each of the two standardized partial regression coefficients exceeds the magnitude of its corresponding standardized bivariate regression coefficient. Classical suppression and cooperative suppression can be considered mutual suppression, but net suppression cannot. Hypothetical and empirical examples of net suppression in which the magnitude of the standardized partial regression coefficient for the suppressor variable is greater than, equal to, and less than the magnitude of its standardized bivariate regression coefficient are provided. The empirical examples were drawn from the social psychology literature relating psychological well-being outcomes to life aspirations.

6.
Med Care ; 42(4): 378-86, 2004 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15076815

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: This study investigated the attitudes of the transplant community toward the current policy of altruistic organ donation and 6 alternative policies offering incentives to the donor family. METHODS: Two hundred forty-nine transplant surgeons, 143 transplant coordinators, and 134 critical-care nurses rated the moral appropriateness and success of the current policy of altruistic donation, rated the moral appropriateness of the alternative policies, judged whether donation would increase or decrease under each alternative policy, and decided whether each alternative policy should be put into effect. RESULTS: All 3 professions believed the current policy of altruistic organ donation to be morally appropriate and most of the alternative policies to be morally appropriate or morally neutral. All 3 professions believed the current policy to be at best only moderately successful and judged most or all of the alternative policies to be likely to increase donation. All 3 professions favored implementing a policy offering donor recognition; approximately one half of the surgeons and one half of the coordinators also advocated implementing a policy offering 1500 US dollars toward funeral expenses. In all 3 professions, respondents' decisions to implement the alternative policies were more frequently related to those policies' moral appropriateness than to their likelihood of increasing organ donation. CONCLUSIONS: Specific incentives varied in their acceptability to the transplant community. Attitudes of the transplant community toward incentives were not in accord with published criticisms of incentives.


Subject(s)
Altruism , Attitude of Health Personnel , Medical Staff, Hospital/psychology , Motivation , Nursing Staff, Hospital/psychology , Tissue and Organ Procurement , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Attitude to Health , Critical Care , Decision Making, Organizational , Female , Funeral Rites , Health Knowledge, Attitudes, Practice , Health Policy , Humans , Logistic Models , Male , Middle Aged , Organizational Policy , Surveys and Questionnaires , Tissue and Organ Procurement/ethics , Tissue and Organ Procurement/organization & administration , United States , Waiting Lists
7.
Psychol Sci ; 14(6): 531-6, 2003 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14629682

ABSTRACT

Recent research has demonstrated that aspiring to the American Dream of financial success has negative consequences for various aspects of psychological well-being. The present longitudinal study examining the relation between the goal for financial success, attainment of that goal, and satisfaction with various life domains found that the negative impact of the goal for financial success on overall life satisfaction diminished as household income increased. The negative consequences of the goal for financial success seemed to be limited to those specific life domains that either concerned relationships with other people or involved income-producing activities, such as one's job; satisfactions with two of those life domains, however, were among the strongest predictors of overall life satisfaction in this sample of well-educated respondents in their late 30s. The negative consequences were particularly severe for the domain of family life; the stronger the goal for financial success, the lower the satisfaction with family life, regardless of household income.


Subject(s)
Aspirations, Psychological , Personal Satisfaction , Quality of Life , Social Class , Culture , Family/psychology , Humans , Regression Analysis , Surveys and Questionnaires , United States
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...