Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 8 de 8
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
2.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 19(6): 1128-34, 2012 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23055140

ABSTRACT

In the present study, we examined the role of attention in modulating the memory benefit of emotional arousal for same-valence word pair associations. To assess the role of attention either at encoding or at retrieval, participants studied lists of positive, neutral, and negative words pairs under full attention, divided attention at encoding, or divided attention at retrieval, and then were tested on the single words and on the associations between words. Consistent with past studies, memory accuracy was higher for emotional items than for neutral items, and no memory difference was observed across emotional arousal conditions for associations when encoding occurred under full attention. In contrast, memory accuracy was higher for emotionally arousing items and associations relative to neutral items when encoding occurred under divided attention. Finally, dividing attention at retrieval revealed similar effects across emotion conditions, suggesting that retrieval of emotional stimuli relative to neutral stimuli, unlike encoding, does not benefit from automatic processing. The discussion emphasizes the role of automatic processing during encoding in producing the benefit of emotionally enhanced memory, as well as the extent to which controlled attention is responsible for eliminating or reversing (relative to neutral materials) emotionally enhanced memory for associations. Additionally, the implications of the divided-attention-at-retrieval manipulation include consideration of the way in which emotional items may be consciously processed during encoding.


Subject(s)
Association Learning/physiology , Attention/physiology , Emotions , Memory, Episodic , Arousal , Female , Humans , Male , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Word Association Tests , Young Adult
3.
Psychol Aging ; 27(2): 462-73, 2012 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21895378

ABSTRACT

Previous research has shown that people-especially older adults-have a special difficulty in learning the names of newly encountered people. This is potentially attributable to the lack of direct link in memory between a face and name. The present experiments investigated whether older and younger adults could use other semantic information about a person (i.e., a "mediator") to indirectly link a name to a face. In each of two experiments, older and younger adults prelearned associations between semantic information (character information or occupations) and names. They then attempted to learn links between faces and either the names or semantic information. In the "unmediated" condition, participants learned only one piece of information (either the name or the semantic information) about each face, whereas in the "mediated" condition, they learned both the to-be-tested information as well as the "mediator" (i.e., both the name and the other semantic information). Experiment 1 showed that, at a simple level, both age groups could use character information ("good" or "bad") to help recognize people's names, given their faces. Experiment 2 showed that knowing the occupation associated with a name helped both age groups to later recall the name associated with a given face.


Subject(s)
Aging/psychology , Association Learning/physiology , Face , Mental Recall , Names , Semantics , Adolescent , Age Factors , Aged , Analysis of Variance , Character , Cues , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Occupations , Practice, Psychological , Reaction Time , Recognition, Psychology , Young Adult
4.
Mem Cognit ; 40(4): 551-66, 2012 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22170485

ABSTRACT

In this study we assessed the potential moderating roles of stimulus type (emotionally arousing) and participants' characteristics (gender) in older adults' associative memory deficit. In two experiments, young and older participants studied lists that included neutral and emotionally arousing word pairs (positive and negative) and completed recognition tests for the words and their associations. In Experiment 1, the majority of the word pairs were composed of two nouns, whereas in Experiment 2 they were composed of adjective-noun pairs. The results extend evidence for older adults' associative deficit and suggest that older and younger adults' item memory is improved for emotionally arousing words. However, associative memory for the word pairs did not benefit (and even showed a slight decline) from emotionally arousing words, which was the case for both younger and older adults. In addition, in these experiments, gender appeared to moderate the associative deficit of older adults, with older males but not females demonstrating this deficit.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Arousal/physiology , Association Learning/physiology , Emotions/physiology , Memory Disorders/physiopathology , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Female , Humans , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Psycholinguistics/methods , Psychological Tests , Sex Factors , Young Adult
5.
Psychol Aging ; 23(2): 467-72, 2008 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18573021

ABSTRACT

The associative deficit hypothesis (M. Naveh-Benjamin, 2000) attributes age-related memory deficits to the inability to encode and retrieve bound units of information. The present experiment extended this deficit to a new form of stimuli, dynamic displays of people and their performance of everyday actions. Older and younger adults viewed a series of brief video clips, each showing a different person performing a different action, and were tested over memory for individual people, individual actions, and the person-action combinations. Older adults did exhibit an associative deficit, and this was related to an increased proportion of false alarms on the associative test.


Subject(s)
Aging/psychology , Association Learning , Interpersonal Relations , Mental Recall , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Attention , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Motion Perception , Psychomotor Performance , Video Recording
6.
Psychol Aging ; 23(1): 104-18, 2008 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18361660

ABSTRACT

In this meta-analysis, the authors evaluated recent suggestions that older adults' episodic memory impairments are partially due to a reduced ability to encode and retrieve associated/bound units of information. Results of 90 studies of episodic memory for both item and associative information in 3,197 young and 3,192 older adults provided support for the age-related associative/binding deficit suggestion, indicating a larger effect of age on memory for associative information than for item information. Moderators assessed included the type of associations, encoding instructions, materials, and test format. Results indicated an age-related associative deficit in memory for source, context, temporal order, spatial location, and item pairings, in both verbal and nonverbal material. An age-related associative deficit was quite pronounced under intentional learning instructions but was not clearly evident under incidental learning instructions. Finally, test format was also found to moderate the associative deficit, with older adults showing an associative/binding deficit when item memory was evaluated via recognition tests but not when item memory was evaluated via recall tests, in which case the age-related deficits were similar for item and associative information.


Subject(s)
Aging/psychology , Association Learning , Mental Recall , Aged , Attention , Humans , Intention , Orientation , Paired-Associate Learning , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Speech Perception
7.
J Proteome Res ; 4(4): 1114-22, 2005.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16083261

ABSTRACT

Proteomics technology and methods remain inadequate. Technological constraints contribute to an artificially static view of complex biological systems and a barrier between quantitative and interaction studies. Several NIH programs combine proteomics technology development with research on challenging biological problems to drive progress. A new initiative of the NIH Roadmap focuses on characterization of dynamic systems. The success of these programs will be judged by their impact on relevant biological problems.


Subject(s)
National Institutes of Health (U.S.) , Proteomics/methods , Biomedical Research , Humans , Proteomics/instrumentation , Technology , United States
8.
Genome Res ; 14(10B): 2121-7, 2004 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15489334

ABSTRACT

The National Institutes of Health's Mammalian Gene Collection (MGC) project was designed to generate and sequence a publicly accessible cDNA resource containing a complete open reading frame (ORF) for every human and mouse gene. The project initially used a random strategy to select clones from a large number of cDNA libraries from diverse tissues. Candidate clones were chosen based on 5'-EST sequences, and then fully sequenced to high accuracy and analyzed by algorithms developed for this project. Currently, more than 11,000 human and 10,000 mouse genes are represented in MGC by at least one clone with a full ORF. The random selection approach is now reaching a saturation point, and a transition to protocols targeted at the missing transcripts is now required to complete the mouse and human collections. Comparison of the sequence of the MGC clones to reference genome sequences reveals that most cDNA clones are of very high sequence quality, although it is likely that some cDNAs may carry missense variants as a consequence of experimental artifact, such as PCR, cloning, or reverse transcriptase errors. Recently, a rat cDNA component was added to the project, and ongoing frog (Xenopus) and zebrafish (Danio) cDNA projects were expanded to take advantage of the high-throughput MGC pipeline.


Subject(s)
Cloning, Molecular/methods , DNA, Complementary , Gene Library , Open Reading Frames/physiology , Animals , Computational Biology , DNA Primers , DNA, Complementary/genetics , DNA, Complementary/metabolism , Humans , Mice , National Institutes of Health (U.S.) , Rats , United States , Xenopus laevis/genetics , Zebrafish/genetics
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...