Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 18 de 18
Filter
1.
Sex Med ; 10(6): 100578, 2022 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36274460

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Similar to other young people with a chronic health condition, perinatally HIV-infected (PHIV) adolescents may have an impacted sexual development. OBJECTIVES: This paper aims to compare sexual milestones of PHIV to HIV uninfected peers, through a systematic review (SR) and explorative study. METHODS: We performed a systematic search in 4 electronic databases (Medline, Embase, Web of Science, and Scopus), according to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines. Last search in all databases was performed in May 2021. We included studies that reported on quantitative data of any of the main outcomes and compared PHIV to HIV uninfected control groups. Main outcomes were defined as the occurrence and/or debut age of sexual milestones (falling in love, having been in a romantic relationship, masturbation, kissing, non-genital caressing (feeling or petting, touching), genital caressing (fingering, handjob), giving or receiving oral sex, and penetrative sex (vaginal or anal). We excluded case reports, audits, guidelines, editorials, abstracts, studies that reported on behaviorally infected HIV patients, studies that did not include an HIV uninfected control group and studies that could not be translated to English or Dutch. We used the Agency for Health Care Research and Quality (AHRQ) Checklist for quality assessment. We performed qualitative synthesis of the data. In the explorative study, we compared sexual milestones of PHIV and HIV uninfected controls matched for age, sex, ethnicity and educational level, using a subset of questions of a validated questionnaire. RESULTS: We included eighteen studies in the SR, describing outcomes of an estimated 1,963 participants. Seventeen studies compared the occurrence and/or debut age of intercourse in PHIV and HIV uninfected controls and 4 studies reported on any of the other sexual milestones. The majority of studies found no difference in occurrence (12 of 16 studies) or debut age (6 of 8 studies) of intercourse in PHIV compared to controls. Two of 4 studies reporting on any of the other milestones found no significant differences between PHIV and HIV uninfected controls. In the explorative study, we included ten PHIV participants and 16 HIV uninfected, matched controls. PHIV tended to report a later debut age of sexual milestones than controls (not significant). STRENGTHS AND LIMITATIONS: The SR includes only a small number of studies and few studies report on non-penetrative milestones. The explorative study adds to this review by including non-penetrative milestones and comparing PHIV to HIV-uninfected, well-matched controls. However, the sample size was small. CONCLUSION: PHIV seem to engage in sexual activities and achieve sexual milestones at a similar rate as their HIV uninfected peers, with a tendency of a later start in well treated PHIV. The review was registered in the PROSPERO database (CRD42021252103) and funded by AIDSfonds. AIDSfonds had no role in the study design or interpretations of this study. ter Haar AM, Fieten A, Van den Hof M, et al. Sexual Development in Perinatally HIV-Infected Young People: A Systematic Review and Explorative Study. Sex Med 2022;10:100578.

2.
AIDS Care ; 34(2): 263-271, 2022 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33793369

ABSTRACT

Young people perinatally infected with HIV (pHIV) are at risk of a lowered health-related quality of life (HRQOL). Previous evaluation of the NeurOlogical, VIsual and Cognitive performance in HIV-infected Children (NOVICE)-cohort showed no difference in HRQOL between pHIV and matched HIV-uninfected controls (HIV-), yet a higher percentage of pHIV had impaired HRQOL. The aim of this study is to compare the change over time in HRQOL of pHIV to HIV- over a 5-year period. We used the Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory (PedsQL)™ 4.0 to repeat HRQOL assessment. High PedsQL scores indicate good HRQOL. Fifteen/33 (45.5%) pHIV and 17/37 (45.9%) HIV- completed both assessments. At the first assessment, the mean age was 13.1 years (range 8.0-18.4). PHIV scored higher than HIV- on Emotional functioning and on Total scale score. After five years, the mean age was 17.6 years (range 12.1-22.8). PHIV scored higher than HIV- on all scales, except Social functioning. PHIV did not differ significantly from the Dutch norm on either time-point. LMEM showed no difference in change over time for any of the PedsQL scales. In this study, young people with pHIV receiving high-quality health care, including monitoring of HRQOL, remain to experience a good HRQOL.


Subject(s)
HIV Infections , Quality of Life , Adolescent , Adult , Child , Cohort Studies , HIV Infections/psychology , Humans , Infectious Disease Transmission, Vertical , Longitudinal Studies , Young Adult
3.
BMC Pediatr ; 21(1): 519, 2021 11 20.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34798840

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Fatigue is common among adults living with human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) as well as children with a chronic disease (CCD). Fatigue can have disastrous effects on health status, including health related quality of life (HRQOL). Even so, fatigue is underexplored in children and adolescents perinatally infected with HIV (PHIV+) in the Netherlands. The objective of this observational study is to explore fatigue in PHIV+ and its association with their HRQOL. METHODS: We measured HRQOL and fatigue using the Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory™ (PedsQL 4.0) and the PedsQL Multidimensional Fatigue Scale (MFS). The PedsQL MFS encompasses three subscales: general fatigue, sleep/rest fatigue and cognitive fatigue, and a total fatigue score. We compared outcomes of PHIV+ children and adolescents in the Amsterdam University Medical Centre with three groups: 1) HIV-uninfected controls (HIV-) matched for age, sex, region of birth, socioeconomic status and adoption status, 2) CCD, and 3) the general Dutch population. Within the PHIV+ group we explored associations between fatigue and HRQOL. RESULTS: We enrolled 14 PHIV+ (median age 10.2 years [IQR 9.2-11.4]) and 14 HIV-. Compared to CCD, PHIV+ significantly reported less general fatigue (mean difference 13.0, 95% CI 1.3 to 24.8). PHIV+ did not score significantly different on any of the other PedsQL MFS scales compared to HIV-, CCD or the general Dutch population. PHIV children scored relatively low on the cognitive fatigue scale in comparison to HIV-uninfected matched controls, CCD and the general population, although these differences did not reach significance. Among PHIV+, a lower score on total fatigue, general fatigue and cognitive fatigue was associated with a lower HRQOL score. CONCLUSIONS: The results of this study suggest that PHIV children and adolescents do not experience more symptoms of fatigue than their healthy peers. However, PHIV children and adolescents may be more likely to experience cognitive fatigue. Fatigue in PHIV also appears to be associated with children's HRQOL. Further research should confirm these exploratory findings.


Subject(s)
HIV Infections , Quality of Life , Adolescent , Adult , Child , Ethnicity , Fatigue/epidemiology , Fatigue/etiology , HIV , HIV Infections/complications , Humans , Infectious Disease Transmission, Vertical
4.
PLoS One ; 14(12): e0224930, 2019.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31805059

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: HIV-associated cognitive deficiency in perinatally HIV-infected (PHIV) children has been studied in Western countries in a population of which an increasing proportion has been internationally adopted. Studies often lack an appropriate internationally adopted HIV-uninfected control group, potentially confounding the relationship between HIV and cognitive functioning. This study aims to further elucidate the association between treated HIV infection and cognitive development by addressing the background of international adoption. METHODS: We cross-sectionally studied the impact of HIV on cognition by comparing PHIV children and HIV- uninfected controls, matched for age-, sex-, ethnicity-, socioeconomic status (SES)- and adoption status. We used a standardized neuropsychological test battery to measure intelligence (IQ), and the cognitive domains of processing speed, working memory, executive function, learning ability and visual-motor function and compared outcomes using lineair regression models, adjusted for IQ. We determined cognitive profiles and cognitive impairment by using multivariate normative comparison (MNC) and explored associations with HIV disease- and treatment-related factors. RESULTS: We enrolled fourteen PHIV children (mean age 10.45 years [1.73 SD], 93% adopted from sub-Saharan Africa at a median age of 3.3 years [IQR 2.1-4.2]) and fifteen HIV- uninfected controls. Groups did not clinically nor statistically differ in age, sex, ethnicity, SES, region of birth, adoption status and age at adoption. PHIV scored consistently lower on all cognitive domains and MNC outcomes. Compared to controls, PHIV children had a significant lower IQ (mean 81 [SD 11] versus mean 97 [SD 15], p = 0.005), and a poorer cognitive profile by MNC (Hotelling's T2 mean -4.36 [SD 5.6] versus mean 0.16 [SD 4.5], p = 0.021), not associated with HIV disease- and treatment-related factors. Two PHIV (14%) and one control (7%) were classified as cognitively impaired (p = 0.598). CONCLUSIONS: Findings indicate treated HIV-infection to be independently associated with lower IQ and poorer cognitive profiles in PHIV children, irrespective of a background of international adoption.


Subject(s)
Anti-HIV Agents/adverse effects , Cognition , HIV Infections/physiopathology , Intellectual Disability/etiology , Adoption , Anti-HIV Agents/therapeutic use , Antiretroviral Therapy, Highly Active , Case-Control Studies , Child , Child, Preschool , Cohort Studies , Cross-Sectional Studies , Female , HIV Infections/congenital , HIV Infections/drug therapy , Humans , Infant , Intelligence Tests , Internationality , Male , Prospective Studies
5.
Eur J Paediatr Neurol ; 22(5): 845-853, 2018 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29735386

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Maternal trauma complicates pregnancy in approximately 7%. Long-term development of children exposed to maternal trauma is unknown. AIM: To determine neurobehavioural outcome of children (6-18 years) born after maternal trauma in pregnancy compared to a matched control group. STUDY DESIGN: Case-control study performed at a tertiary medical centre. SUBJECTS: All consecutive children born after maternal hospitalization for trauma during pregnancy between 1995 and 2005. Controls were children born at the same hospital and period after an uneventful pregnancy. OUTCOME MEASURES: Trauma type and severity (Injury Severity Score, ≥9: severe); information from medical files at admission (cases). All mothers filled out two questionnaires about the infant; 1. concerning health, motor development and educational level, 2. concerning behavioural development through the validated Dutch version of the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL). RESULTS: Questionnaires were returned by 34 cases and 28 controls. The traumas concerned mainly motor vehicle accidents and falls, and 3/34 had severe injuries. No differences in health, motor development, educational level and CBCL was found between the cases and controls, except for more hospitalization in the cases (p = 0.009). CONCLUSION: Long-term follow-up of a limited population of children 6-18 years after exposure of mainly non-severe trauma before birth is similar to a control population except for unexplained more hospitalization in the cases.


Subject(s)
Developmental Disabilities/epidemiology , Maternal Exposure/adverse effects , Prenatal Exposure Delayed Effects , Accidental Falls , Accidents, Traffic , Adolescent , Adult , Case-Control Studies , Child , Child Development/physiology , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Infant , Male , Pregnancy , Surveys and Questionnaires
6.
Child Neuropsychol ; 24(6): 844-858, 2018 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28693404

ABSTRACT

Pediatric brain tumor survivors (PBTS) suffer from cognitive late effects, such as deteriorating executive functioning (EF). We explored the suitability of the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) to screen for these late effects. We assessed the relationship between the BRIEF and EF tasks, and between the BRIEF-Parent and BRIEF-Teacher, and we explored the clinical utility. Eighty-two PBTS (8-18 years) were assessed with EF tasks measuring attention, cognitive flexibility, inhibition, visual-, and working memory (WM), and with the BRIEF-Parent and BRIEF-Teacher. Pearson's correlations between the BRIEF and EF tasks, and between the BRIEF-Parent and BRIEF-Teacher were calculated. The BRIEF-Parent related poorly to EF tasks (rs < .26, ps > .01), but of the BRIEF-Teacher the WM-scale, Monitor-scale, Behavioral-Regulation-Index, and Meta-cognition-Index, and Total-score (rs > .31, ps < .01) related significantly to some EF tasks. When controlling for age, only the WM scale and Total score related significantly to the attention task (ps < .01). The inhibit scales of the BRIEF-Parent and BRIEF-Teacher correlated significantly (r = .33, p < .01). Children with clinically elevated scores on BRIEF scales that correlated with EF tasks performed worse on all EF tasks (ds 0.56-1.23, ps < .05). The BRIEF-Teacher Total and Index scores might better screen general EF in PBTS than the BRIEF-Parent. However, the BRIEF-Teacher is also not specific enough to capture separate EFs. Solely relying on the BRIEF as a screening measure of EFs in BPTS is insufficient. Questionnaires and tasks give distinctive, valuable information.


Subject(s)
Brain Neoplasms/diagnosis , Cognition/physiology , Executive Function/physiology , Health Behavior/physiology , Neuropsychological Tests , Adolescent , Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/psychology , Brain Neoplasms/mortality , Brain Neoplasms/pathology , Child , Female , Humans , Male , Survival Rate
7.
Horm Res Paediatr ; 73(1): 6-14, 2010.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20190535

ABSTRACT

Intrauterine growth restriction (IUGR) can lead to infants being born small for gestational age (SGA). SGA is associated with increased neonatal morbidity and mortality as well as short stature, cardiovascular disease, insulin resistance, diabetes mellitus type 2, dyslipidemia and end-stage renal disease in adulthood. In addition, SGA children have decreased levels of intelligence and cognition, although the effects are mostly subtle. The overall outcome of each child is the result of a complex interaction between intrauterine and extrauterine factors. Animal and human studies show structural alterations in the brains of individuals with IUGR/SGA. The presence of growth hormone (GH) receptors in the brain implies that the brain is also a target for GH. Exogenous GH theoretically has the ability to act on the brain. This is exemplified by the effects of GH on cognition in GH-deficient adults. In SGA children, data on the effect of exogenous GH on intelligence and cognition are scant and contradictory.


Subject(s)
Brain/growth & development , Child Development/physiology , Cognition/physiology , Infant, Small for Gestational Age/growth & development , Intelligence/physiology , Adult , Brain/physiology , Child , Fetal Growth Retardation/physiopathology , Humans , Infant, Newborn , Infant, Small for Gestational Age/physiology , Models, Biological , Outcome Assessment, Health Care
8.
Psychooncology ; 18(1): 87-95, 2009 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18613302

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To assess behavioural problems in retinoblastoma (RB) survivors. METHODS: This population-based cross-sectional study included 148 RB survivors (8-35 years), registered in the Dutch national RB register. Survivors and parents were asked to fill in behavioural questionnaires. Prevalence rates were computed, based on both self-reports and proxy reports. One-sample T-tests were applied to analyse differences compared with healthy reference samples. Multiple regression analyses were performed to identify predictors for behavioural problems within the RB sample. RESULTS: Between-group differences varied across informants and across age groups. Parents reported significantly elevated total problem behaviour in 30% of their offspring (aged 8-17 years); this against 9% in adolescents (12-17 years) and 12% in adults (18-35 years) based on self-report. Parental reports showed significantly elevated rates of (1) internalising problems in boys and (2) somatic complaints in both girls and boys. Self-reports indicate significantly lowered levels of (1) externalising problems in adolescent and adult women and (2) thought problems in female adolescents and in adult men. Especially survivors who suffered hereditary RB, who had undergone more intensive treatment, and who came from a single-parent family were identified to be at most behavioural risk. CONCLUSION: Perception of severity and the nature of behavioural problems seem to differ between beholder, and to vary between age groups, if not between life stages. Health professionals should be aware that especially those who are confronted with hereditary RB and who subsequently undergo intensive treatment, and who grow up in broken families, run the risk of developing behavioural difficulties.


Subject(s)
Child Behavior Disorders/epidemiology , Retinal Neoplasms/rehabilitation , Retinoblastoma/rehabilitation , Social Behavior Disorders/epidemiology , Survivors/psychology , Adolescent , Adult , Age Distribution , Case-Control Studies , Child , Cross-Sectional Studies , Female , Humans , Male , Multivariate Analysis , Netherlands/epidemiology , Prevalence , Regression Analysis , Retinal Neoplasms/psychology , Retinoblastoma/psychology , Risk Factors , Sex Distribution
9.
Brain ; 128(Pt 7): 1546-55, 2005 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15817514

ABSTRACT

A 3.5-year follow-up study of cognition and behaviour in 42 children with newly diagnosed idiopathic or cryptogenic epilepsy ('epilepsy only') attending mainstream education and 30 healthy gender-matched classmate controls was carried out to identify differences between groups, to detect factors that contribute to the difference and its change over time, and to establish the proportion of poorly performing children. The neuropsychological battery covered the major domains of cognition, mental and motor speed and academic language skills. Children were tested at the time of diagnosis (before any anti-epileptic drug treatment started) and 3, 12 and approximately 42 months later. Parents and teachers completed behaviour checklists, for which the scoring was adapted to prevent any influence of epilepsy-related ambiguity. Based on parental interviews at the time of diagnosis, children with epilepsy were categorized as having longstanding behavioural and/or learning problems, as belonging to a troubled family, as being exposed to 'off-balance' parenting starting at the time of epilepsy onset and/or as reacting maladaptively to the changes in relation to the onset of epilepsy. Throughout follow-up, the group of children with epilepsy only performed less well than healthy classmates on measures of learning, memory span for words, attention and behaviour. After controlling for school delay, proactive interference (number of responses to the same images as in the learning trials, but now presented in reordered locations) was the only remaining variable that distinguished the group of children with epilepsy only. Group-wise, no changes in cognitive and behavioural differences over time were found, but instability in individual performances appeared to characterize children with epilepsy only. Rather than intrinsically epilepsy-related variables, such as idiopathic versus cryptogenic aetiology, seizure control or anti-epileptic drug treatment, the child's prediagnostic learning and behavioural histories and the parents' ability to continue their habitual parenting in the face of the diagnosis of epilepsy only were shown by both group-wise and case-by-case analyses to be important for understanding the cognitive and behavioural functioning of the children with epilepsy only.


Subject(s)
Child Behavior , Cognition , Epilepsy/psychology , Adaptation, Psychological , Adolescent , Case-Control Studies , Chi-Square Distribution , Child , Female , Follow-Up Studies , Humans , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Parenting , Prospective Studies , Schools , Time Factors
11.
Dev Med Child Neurol ; 44(12): 803-11, 2002 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12455856

ABSTRACT

The aim of the study was to determine whether learning and memory are compromised in school children with recently diagnosed idiopathic and/or cryptogenic epilepsy and to study relationships between learning and memory and psychosocial and epilepsy variables. Word span and learning of locations were assessed within 48 hours after diagnosis of epilepsy and three and 12 months later, in 69 school children with epilepsy (aged 9.1 years, SD 2.7; 33 males, 36 females) and 66 classmates. Results showed that patients and controls performed similarly in registration, recall, and retention. Patients recalled slightly less than controls when probed under conditions of increased demand on working memory. Maladaptive reactions of parents and children to the onset of epilepsy and not reaching 6-months of seizure remission contributed to poor performance. Individually, those patients who required special assistance at school, under-performed occasionally in one or the other component of memory. Although the proportion of under-performers was stable over time, the children composing the group did change. It was concluded that school children with new onset idiopathic or cryptogenic epilepsy are inordinately vulnerable when processing memory tasks. The vulnerability is neither persistent nor memory-specific.


Subject(s)
Epilepsy/complications , Learning Disabilities/etiology , Memory Disorders/etiology , Child , Controlled Clinical Trials as Topic , Female , Follow-Up Studies , Humans , Learning Disabilities/diagnosis , Learning Disabilities/epidemiology , Male , Memory Disorders/diagnosis , Memory Disorders/epidemiology , Neuropsychological Tests , Prospective Studies , Severity of Illness Index , Underachievement
12.
Epilepsia ; 43(3): 301-10, 2002 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11906516

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: To compare problems of attention in schoolchildren with newly diagnosed idiopathic or cryptogenic epilepsy with those in healthy classmates. METHODS: A computerized battery of tasks comprised Reaction Time (RT) measurement, Trail making (Color Trails 1 and 2), Manual Tapping and Steadiness, and a newly developed task of sustained attention (Balloon Piercing). SUBJECTS: Fifty-one children with epilepsy (age 7-16 years) and 48 gender- and age-matched classmates were assessed thrice: within 48 h after diagnosis [before start of antiepileptic drugs (AEDs)], and 3 and 12 months later. Significantly more children with epilepsy (51%) than control children (27%) had required special educational assistance at school. RESULTS: Children with epilepsy could not be distinguished from controls in execution times or motor speed. However, errors were more frequent among patients in a "go-no-go" RT task, and errors of omission in a task requiring sustained attention. Within the group of children with epilepsy, those with prior school or behavior difficulties and those whose parents reacted maladaptively to the onset and diagnosis of epilepsy performed worse than those without these adversities, in the sense that their RT increased inordinately with increasing task difficulty. Epilepsy-related variables did not explain any variance. Transient inordinately poor performances were found in 69% of patients and 40% of controls. CONCLUSIONS: Children with newly diagnosed "epilepsy only" do not have persistent attention deficits. AED treatment has no detrimental effect on attention. Prior school and behavior difficulties and a maladaptive reaction to the onset of epilepsy rather than epilepsy variables are related to decreased attentional efficiency.


Subject(s)
Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/etiology , Epilepsy/psychology , Attention , Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/physiopathology , Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/psychology , Child , Humans , Motor Activity , Neuropsychological Tests , Reaction Time , Reference Values
14.
Epileptic Disord ; 3(1): 39-45, 2001.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11313222

ABSTRACT

Although the child behavior checklist (CBCL) and the teacher's report form (TRF) were not designed for diagnosing psychopathology in children with chronic illnesses, they have become extensively used research tools to assess behavioural problems in paediatric populations, including children with epilepsy. When applied to children with epilepsy, items like "staring blankly" or "twitching" can be rated on the basis of seizure features rather than behaviour and, hence, render behavioural scores ambiguous. The aims were detection, and evaluation of the impact, of CBCL and TRF items eliciting ambiguity when applied to children with "epilepsy only" (idiopathic or cryptogenic epilepsy, attending normal schools). Experts identified items that give rise to interpretational ambiguity of the ratings in epilepsy. By treating ratings on these items as missing values, their effect was evaluated in CBCL and TRF scores of 59 schoolchildren with "epilepsy only" and age and gender matched healthy classmates. Seven items of the CBCL gave rise to ambiguity of which items 5 co-occur on the TRF. Rescoring reduced psychopathology scores in children with "epilepsy only", but not in those of healthy children: the percentage of patients trespassing the clinical cut off score, on at least one of the subscales, reduced from 46 to 23% on the CBCL and from 18 to 15% on the TRF. Parents and teachers run the risk of confusing behaviour and seizure features when filling out the CBCL and TRF. In "epilepsy only", prevalence estimates of psychopathology based on the CBCL and TRF, should be considered with some reserve.


Subject(s)
Child Behavior Disorders/diagnosis , Child Behavior Disorders/etiology , Epilepsy/complications , Attention/physiology , Child , Child Behavior Disorders/psychology , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Male , Severity of Illness Index , Surveys and Questionnaires , Thinking/physiology
15.
Epilepsia ; 42(11): 1452-60, 2001 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11879349

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: We report the parent's perceptions of and reactions to the onset of "epilepsy only" and the implications for continuity of parenting. METHODS: Content analysis was used to extract data on perceived (dis)continuity of parenting, from interviews held with parents of 69 schoolchildren in whom idiopathic or cryptogenic epilepsy ("epilepsy only") had recently been diagnosed. RESULTS: Almost half of the parents (42%) perceived neither themselves nor their child as having been thrown off balance by the onset of epilepsy. Quite a few parents (33%) perceived themselves rather than their child as having been thrown off balance. More parents of children with cryptogenic than with idiopathic epilepsy perceived themselves as being off balance. However, parents' perceptions of their children's reactions to the epilepsy-related changes were not influenced by any epilepsy variable. Rather, family trouble, long-standing behavioral problems, and adolescence contributed to the child's maladaptive reaction. Three extremely off-balance parents had children with seizures of "unclassifiable" epilepsy that later were found to be nonepileptic and psychogenic. CONCLUSIONS: The majority of the parents perceived their child as adapting well to the onset of epilepsy only. Children with seizure onset in adolescence and children with other adversities were perceived as adapting poorly to the additional adversity of epilepsy. Cryptogenic rather than idiopathic etiology leaves parents in great suspense. In behavioral studies, it is advisable to treat children with unclassifiable epilepsy as a separate group.


Subject(s)
Attitude to Health , Epilepsy/epidemiology , Family Health , Parents/psychology , Perception , Adolescent , Adult , Age of Onset , Child , Child Behavior Disorders/epidemiology , Child Behavior Disorders/psychology , Child, Preschool , Epilepsy/diagnosis , Epilepsy/psychology , Female , Humans , Interviews as Topic/methods , Learning Disabilities/epidemiology , Learning Disabilities/psychology , Male , Netherlands/epidemiology , Parenting/psychology
16.
Dev Med Child Neurol ; 42(6): 392-7, 2000 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10875524

ABSTRACT

Set-shifting is an important aspect of attention and regulation of behaviour. The ability of young children to shift set is debated. Set-shifting is usually examined using sorting tasks that are designed for adults and hence are inadequate for children. In this study, an experimental Sorting Task for Children (STC) is administered to 54 healthy children (aged 4 to 10 years) and compared with a widely used adult sorting task, the Modified Card Sorting Test. This experiment shows that preschool children are capable of set-shifting. An extensive error analysis of the STC reveals that perseverations are not predominant. Hence, set-shifting in young children may be characterised by 'trial and error' rather than by being stuck in a mental set, which is suggested to be typical of adults with frontal lobe damage. A pilot study of 10 children with idiopathic or cryptogenic epilepsy shows unstable set-shifting rather than inability to shift set, which is concordant with known problems in regulation of behaviour.


Subject(s)
Epilepsy/physiopathology , Epilepsy/psychology , Neuropsychological Tests , Set, Psychology , Age Factors , Attention , Behavior , Child , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Male , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Pilot Projects , Predictive Value of Tests
17.
Epilepsia ; 41(3): 326-31, 2000 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10714405

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: To understand the emotional predicament in children with recently diagnosed idiopathic or cryptogenic epilepsy. METHODS: We used the well-tried method of structured projection for the first time in children with epilepsy. Thirty-six children with epilepsy, aged 7-15 years (mean age, 9.5 years) and in 35 control children aged 7-15 years (mean age, 9.4 years), attributed shame and guilt in relation to three types of situation (non-illness related, illness related, and epilepsy related). Children were evaluated twice: shortly after diagnosis, before antiepileptic drug (AED) use and after an interval of 3 months. RESULTS: Children with epilepsy and healthy controls were similar in their way of attributing shame and guilt. However, the type of situation was of influence: Both children with epilepsy and healthy children attributed more shame to incompetence due to epilepsy than to incompetence due to other illnesses. CONCLUSIONS: Increased affective problems in childhood epilepsy cannot be explained by excessive attribution of shame and guilt, affects known to be important precursors of psychopathology, yet both healthy children and children with epilepsy attribute more shame to epilepsy than to other illnesses. Epilepsy is not like any other disease.


Subject(s)
Epilepsy/psychology , Guilt , Shame , Adolescent , Affective Symptoms/diagnosis , Affective Symptoms/psychology , Age Factors , Attitude to Health , Child , Epilepsy/diagnosis , Female , Humans , Judgment , Male , Projective Techniques/statistics & numerical data , Sex Factors
18.
Isr J Med Sci ; 30(12): 897-901, 1994 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8002272

ABSTRACT

Dermatography is the application of tattooing techniques in medicine. In the past, several techniques were tried but none led to reproducible results. Dermatography has been developed over the last 10 years into a technique which is both generally applicable and reproducible. It is a suitable treatment modality for disturbing discolorations and scars of the skin which can be permanently camouflaged.


Subject(s)
Dermatology , Tattooing/trends , Humans , Pigments, Biological , Tattooing/instrumentation , Tattooing/methods
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...