Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 7 de 7
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Psychol Med ; 35(6): 807-16, 2005 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15997601

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: A lack of inhibitory control has been suggested to be the core deficit in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). This means that a primary deficit in behavioral inhibition mediates a cascade of secondary deficits in other executive functions, such as arousal regulation. Clinical observations have revealed that with increasing age symptoms of hyperactivity and impulsivity decline at a higher rate than those of inattention. This might imply that a deficit in attention rather than a lack of inhibitory control is the major feature in adult ADHD. METHOD: To study whether an attentional or inhibitory deficit predominates, the stop-signal task and the stop-change task were presented to 24 adults with ADHD combined subtype and 24 controls. RESULTS: Relative to controls, the stop-signal reaction time (SSRT) was significantly more prolonged than the go-stimulus reaction time (RT) in patients with ADHD. This disproportionate elongation of the SSRT was comparable across tasks, even though the stop-change task exerted more complex (or at least different) demands on the inhibitory system than the stop-signal task. ADHD patients had a higher proportion of choice errors, possibly reflecting more premature responses. Specifically in the stop-change task, patients had more variable choice responses and made more inappropriate change responses, which may also reflect enhanced impulsivity. CONCLUSIONS: The results support a core deficit in behavioral inhibition in adults with ADHD. We further suggest that there is more evidence for a critical role of deficient inhibitory control in adults than in children with ADHD.


Subject(s)
Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/complications , Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/psychology , Cognition Disorders/etiology , Inhibition, Psychological , Reaction Time , Adolescent , Adult , Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/diagnosis , Cognition Disorders/diagnosis , Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Severity of Illness Index
2.
Int J Psychophysiol ; 58(1): 59-70, 2005 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15950304

ABSTRACT

The present selective review addresses attention, inhibition, and their underlying brain mechanisms, especially in relation to attention deficit/hyperactivity disorders (AD/HD), and the effects of methylphenidate. In particular, event-related potential (ERP) studies suggest a deficit in the early-filtering aspect of selective attention in children with AD/HD. Results from stop tasks are consistent with impairments in stopping performance in AD/HD, but in children (as opposed to adults) these effects cannot be easily dissociated from more general impairments in attention to the task, and therefore an interpretation in terms of inhibitory control is not straightforward. On the other hand, the beneficial effects of methylphenidate are more specific to stopping, and there are no clearcut effects of methylphenidate on measures of selective attention. Even when group differences pertain specifically to stopping performance (as with adults with AD/HD), ERP evidence suggests at least a partial contribution of differences in switching attention to the stop signal, as revealed in measures of sensory cortex activation. ERP evidence from cued go/nogo tasks underlines the importance of taking into account the contribution of higher order control processes involved in anticipation of and preparation for task stimuli. It suggests that in certain conditions, expectancy, rather than response bias, contributes to increased behavioral response tendencies, and that a presumed index of response inhibition, the nogo N2, may rather reflect conflict monitoring. In sum, direct reflections of brain activity suggest that mechanisms of expectation and attention, rather than of response bias or inhibitory control, govern behavioral manifestations of impulsivity.


Subject(s)
Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/drug therapy , Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/physiopathology , Central Nervous System Stimulants/therapeutic use , Impulsive Behavior/drug therapy , Impulsive Behavior/physiopathology , Adult , Child , Evoked Potentials , Humans
3.
Behav Brain Res ; 145(1-2): 7-15, 2003 Oct 17.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14529800

ABSTRACT

The objective of this study was to investigate the effects of methylphenidate (MPH) on attention and inhibition in children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and to establish what the relative contributions of the noradrenergic and dopaminergic systems to this effect were. In addition to MPH, two other drugs were administered in order to affect both transmitter systems more selectively, L-dopa (dopamine (DA) agonist) and desipramine (DMI) (noradrenaline (NA) re-uptake inhibitor). Sixteen children with ADHD performed a stop-task, a laboratory task that measures the ability to inhibit an ongoing action, in a double-blind randomized within-subjects design. Each child received an acute clinical dose of MPH, DMI, L-dopa, and placebo; measures of performance and plasma were determined. The results indicated that inhibition performance was improved under DMI but not under MPH or L-dopa. The response-time to the stop-signal was marginally shortened after intake of DMI. MPH decreased omission and choice-errors and caused faster reaction times to the trials without the stop-tone. No effects of L-dopa whatsoever were noted. Prolactin levels were increased and 5-HIAA levels were lowered under DMI relative to placebo. It is suggested that the effects of MPH on attention are due to a combination of noradrenergic and dopaminergic mechanisms. The improved inhibition under DMI could be serotonergically mediated.


Subject(s)
Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/drug therapy , Attention/drug effects , Desipramine/therapeutic use , Dopamine Agents/therapeutic use , Enzyme Inhibitors/therapeutic use , Inhibition, Psychological , Levodopa/therapeutic use , Methylphenidate/therapeutic use , Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/blood , Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/physiopathology , Child , Choice Behavior/drug effects , Desipramine/analogs & derivatives , Desipramine/blood , Double-Blind Method , Enzyme Inhibitors/blood , Humans , Hydroxyindoleacetic Acid/blood , Male , Prolactin/blood , Reaction Time/drug effects
4.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 75(4): 263-90, 2000 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10698613

ABSTRACT

When children grow older they respond faster and are less susceptible to interference caused by task-irrelevant information. These observations suggested the hypothesis that a global mechanism may account for developmental change in the speed of responding and that inhibitory function may underlie the ability to activate speeded responses. The current study examined these issues by comparing the performance of 4 age groups (5-, 8-, and 11-year-olds and young adults) on a battery of 6 speeded performance tasks, 4 of which required the inhibition of response activation. An analysis of reaction and inhibition times supported a hypothesis of generalized developmental changes in response activation, but revealed a less pronounced development of inhibition. A nonselective mechanism of response inhibition seems to be fully developed during early childhood.


Subject(s)
Child Development/physiology , Inhibition, Psychological , Adult , Age Factors , Auditory Perception/physiology , Child , Child, Preschool , Cognition/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Reaction Time
5.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry ; 37(9): 977-85, 1998 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9735617

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: First, to differentiate between inattention and impulsivity based on type of errors made in the AX version of the Continuous Performance Task (CPT), and second, to investigate whether differences in performance between children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and normal controls also occur in specific forms of brain activity, namely event-related potentials (ERPs), presumably related to inattention and impulsivity or inhibition. METHOD: Sixteen ADHD and 16 normal control children performed the CPT-AX. ERPs were recorded at occipital (Oz), parietal (Pz), central (Cz), and frontal (Fz) leads. RESULTS: The ADHD children had a higher CPT-Inattention score and showed smaller parietal positive waves at a latency of approximately 300 msec in reaction to target stimuli, target P3s, likewise indicating less attention. In contrast, they showed neither higher CPT-Impulsivity nor a smaller frontocentral negative wave at about 200 msec (N2); the N2 is generally seen as reflecting inhibition. A subgroup of children with ADHD and oppositional defiant disorder (n = 6) had smaller N2 waves than controls, however. CONCLUSIONS: The ADHD group studied showed deficits in attention but not in impulsivity (or inhibition).


Subject(s)
Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/physiopathology , Attention/physiology , Cerebral Cortex/physiopathology , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Impulsive Behavior/physiopathology , Inhibition, Psychological , Adolescent , Analysis of Variance , Case-Control Studies , Child , Female , Humans , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Regression Analysis
6.
Behav Brain Res ; 94(1): 97-110, 1998 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9708843

ABSTRACT

A total of 11 children with attention deficit disorder (ADD) and nine control children performed a continuous performance test (CPT) of the A-X type with concurrent neuroelectric brain mapping to assess preparatory processing, purportedly mediated by the frontal lobes. This cued CPT task proved to be a highly specific task. The groups could be clearly differentiated both at the behavioral and electrophysiological level. ADD children detected fewer signals and made more false alarms. There were no major group differences in topographical distribution of the event-related potential microstates, but ADD children displayed reduced global field power (GFP) in an early CNV/P3 microstate to cues. This indicated that impaired orienting to cues, rather than impaired executive target processing, determines the initial processing stages in ADD. In comparison with data from the same task run in Utrecht, the same orienting deficit in clinically diagnosed ADHD children was demonstrated. Low resolution electromagnetic tomography (LORETA) estimated posterior sources underlying these orienting processes and the orienting deficit. This argued against frontal lobe involvement at this stage and suggested involvement of a posterior attention system.


Subject(s)
Arousal/physiology , Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/physiopathology , Attention/physiology , Brain Mapping , Frontal Lobe/physiopathology , Neuropsychological Tests , Orientation/physiology , Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/diagnosis , Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/psychology , Child , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Reference Values , Signal Processing, Computer-Assisted
7.
J Abnorm Child Psychol ; 22(5): 561-78, 1994 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7822629

ABSTRACT

Although it has frequently been reported that hyperactive children have abnormally small P3 amplitudes of the event-related potential (ERP), which are normalized by the stimulant drug methylphenidate (MPH), the literature is inconsistent concerning earlier ERP waves. The aim of the present study was to investigate whether the normalizing effect of a 10-mg dose of MPH was also apparent on earlier waves, such as the N1, the P2, and the N2, besides the P3. Twelve attention deficit with hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) children performed a Continuous Performance Test involving a button-press response to the letter X (CPT-X) under the influence of MPH in a double-blind placebo controlled acute dosage design. ERPs were recorded at Oz, Pz, Cz, and Fz. The expected increase of the parietal P3, both to targets and nontargets, was apparent, as well as a significant increase in percentage of hits. There also was a significant increase of an earlier, negative going, wave, the N2, with a frontal maximum, under the influence of MPH. This wave was probably a manifestation of an increase in processing negativity for target stimuli only, after the intake of the stimulant drug. No effect of MPH was found on the N1 or the P2.


Subject(s)
Arousal/drug effects , Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/drug therapy , Attention/drug effects , Electroencephalography/drug effects , Methylphenidate/therapeutic use , Psychomotor Performance/drug effects , Adolescent , Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/psychology , Cerebral Cortex/drug effects , Child , Double-Blind Method , Evoked Potentials, Visual/drug effects , Female , Humans , Male , Methylphenidate/adverse effects , Reaction Time/drug effects
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...