Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 13 de 13
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis ; 35(4): B11-B18, 2018 Apr 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29603933

ABSTRACT

We used triple silent substitution stimuli to characterize human S-cone electroretinograms (ERGs) in normal trichromats. Short-wavelength-cone (S-cone) ERGs were found to have different morphological features and temporal frequency response characteristics compared to ERGs derived from L-cones, M-cones, and rod photoreceptors in normal participants. Furthermore, in two cases of retinal pathology, blue cone monochromatism (BCM) and enhanced S-cone syndrome (ESCS), S-cone ERGs elicited by our stimuli were preserved and enhanced, respectively. The results from both normal and pathological retinae demonstrate that triple silent substitution stimuli can be used to generate ERGs that provide an assay of human S-cone function.


Subject(s)
Color Vision Defects/physiopathology , Cone Opsins/physiology , Electroretinography , Eye Diseases, Hereditary/physiopathology , Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Retinal Degeneration/physiopathology , Vision Disorders/physiopathology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Photic Stimulation , Young Adult
2.
Doc Ophthalmol ; 134(1): 11-24, 2017 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28091887

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: To record transient ERGs from the light-adapted human retina using silent substitution stimuli which selectively reflect the activity of rod photoreceptors. We aim to describe the morphology of these waveforms and examine how they are affected by the use of less selective stimuli and by retinal pathology. METHODS: Rod-isolating stimuli with square-wave temporal profiles (250/250 ms onset/offset) were presented using a 4 primary LED ganzfeld stimulator. Experiment 1: ERGs were recorded using a rod-isolating stimulus (63 ph Td, rod contrast, C rod = 0.25) from a group (n = 20) of normal trichromatic observers. Experiment 2: Rod ERGs were recorded from a group (n = 5) using a rod-isolating stimulus (C rod = 0.25) which varied in retinal illuminance from 40 to 10,000 ph Td. Experiment 3: ERGs were elicited using 2 kinds of non-isolating stimuli; (1) broadband and (2) rod-isolating stimuli which contained varying degrees of L- and M-cone excitation. Experiment 4: Rod ERGs were recorded from two patient groups with rod monochromacy (n = 3) and CSNB (type 1; n = 2). RESULTS: The rod-isolated ERGs elicited from normal subjects had a waveform with a positive onset component followed by a negative offset. Response amplitude was maximal at retinal illuminances <100 ph Td and was virtually abolished at 400 ph Td. The use of non-selective stimuli altered the ERG waveform eliciting more photopic-like ERG responses. Rod ERGs recorded from rod monochromats had similar features to those recorded from normal trichromats, in contrast to those recorded from participants with CSNB which had an electronegative appearance. CONCLUSIONS: Our results demonstrate that ERGs elicited by silent substitution stimuli can selectively reflect the operation of rod photoreceptors in the normal, light-adapted human retina.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Electroretinography/methods , Retina/physiology , Retinal Rod Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Photic Stimulation/methods , Young Adult
3.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt ; 30(5): 503-10, 2010 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20883333

ABSTRACT

We recorded L- and M-cone isolating ERGs from human subjects using a silent substitution technique at temporal rates of 12 and 30 Hz. These frequencies isolate the activity of cone-opponent and non-opponent post-receptoral mechanisms, respectively. ERGs were obtained using a sequence of stimuli with different spatial configurations comprising; (1) circular stimuli of different sizes which increased in 10° steps up to 70°diameter, or (2) annular stimuli with a 70° outer diameter but with different sized central ablations from 10° up to 60°. L- and M-cone isolating ERGs were obtained from five colour normal subjects using a DTL fibre electrode. Fourier analysis of the ERGs was performed and we measured the amplitude of the first harmonic of the response. For 12 Hz ERGs the L:M cone response amplitude ratio (L:M(ERG)) was close to unity and remained stable irrespective of the spatial configuration of the stimulus. The maintenance of this balanced ratio points to the existence of cone selective input across the human retina for the L-M cone opponent mechanism. For 30 Hz the L:M(ERG) ratio was greater than unity but varied depending upon which region of the retina was being stimulated. This variation we consider to be a consequence of the global response properties of M-cone ERGs rather than representing a real variation in L:M cone ratios across the retina.


Subject(s)
Color Vision/physiology , Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Electroretinography/methods , Flicker Fusion , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation/methods , Retina/physiology
4.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt ; 30(5): 553-9, 2010 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20883339

ABSTRACT

It is known that there is a distortion of hue and saturation in the peripheral visual field. In a previous study, when an asymmetric matching paradigm was used, four hues in the blue, red, yellow and green regions of colour space were unchanged and these were referred to as peripherally invariant (Parry et al., J Opt Soc Am A, 23, 2006, 1586). Three of these invariant hues were similar to unique blue, red and yellow. However, for most observers there was a marked difference between unique and invariant green. To investigate this apparent paradox, we have measured unique hues using a range of eccentricities and colourimetric purities. An asymmetric matching and a 4-AFC paradigm were used to establish peripherally invariant and unique hues, respectively. In the asymmetric matching task the observer matched a peripheral spot with a para-foveal spot, for 24 different hues at 18° eccentricity. In the 4-AFC paradigm, 41 hues were presented 20 times at three purities (0.5, 0.75 and 1.0) and three eccentricities (18°, 10° and 1°). The observer had to name the hues as red, blue, green or yellow. Unique hues were found to be constant with eccentricity and purity. The unique green, established with 4-AFC, was found to differ from the invariant green, determined using the matching task. However, red, blue and yellow invariant hues correspond well with unique hues. The data suggest that different mechanisms mediate the matching of green compared with the identification of unique hues. This is similar to the difference between detection and discrimination of spectral stimuli: the detection process is dominated by the cone opponent mechanisms and is most sensitive, whereas more central processes, serving unique hues, influence discrimination.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Adult , Confounding Factors, Epidemiologic , Fovea Centralis/physiology , Humans , Photic Stimulation/methods , Psychophysics , Visual Fields/physiology
5.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt ; 30(5): 594-601, 2010 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20883344

ABSTRACT

We examined visual short term memory (VSTM) for colour using a delayed-match-to-sample paradigm. In these experiments we measured the effects of increasing inter-stimulus interval (ISI), varying between 0 and 10 s, on the ability of five colour normal human observers to make colour matches between a reference and subsequently presented test stimuli. The coloured stimuli used were defined by different chromatic axes on the isoluminant plane of DKL colour space. In preliminary experiments we used a hue scaling procedure to identify a total of 12 colour stimuli which served as reference hues in the colour memory experiments: four stimuli were exemplars of red, green, blue and yellow colour appearance categories, four were located between these categories and a further four were located on the cardinal axes that isolated the activity of the cone-opponent mechanisms. Our results demonstrate that there is a reduction in the ability of observers to make accurate colour matches with increasing ISIs and that this reduced performance was similar for all colour stimuli. However, the shifts in hue that were measured between the reference and matched test stimuli were significantly greater for the cardinal stimuli compared to those measured for the stimuli defined by the hue scaling procedure. This deterioration in the retention of hue in VSTM for stimuli that isolate cone-opponent mechanisms may be a reflection of the reorganisation of colour processing that occurs in the cortex where colour appearance mechanisms become more prominent.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation/methods , Psychophysics , Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Time Factors
6.
Ophthalmic Physiol Opt ; 29(3): 375-81, 2009 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19422572

ABSTRACT

The purpose of this study is to establish whether nasal-temporal differences in cone photoreceptor distributions are linked to differences in colour matching performance in the two hemi-fields. Perceived shifts in chromaticity were measured using an asymmetric matching paradigm. They were expressed in terms of hue rotations and relative saturation changes and also in terms of activation levels of L-M or S-(L+M) cone-opponent channels. Up to 19 degrees eccentricity there was little difference in chromaticity shifts between nasal and temporal retina for either channel. For matches beyond 19 degrees L-M activation is significantly lower in the nasal field and the S-(L+M) channel was equally activated in both fields. The data are consistent with the asymmetric distribution of L- and M-cones in the nasal and temporal retinae.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Retina/physiology , Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Visual Fields/physiology , Color Perception Tests/methods , Humans , Photic Stimulation/methods
7.
Vision Res ; 49(16): 2056-66, 2009 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19460399

ABSTRACT

Many classical experiments have shown that two superimposed gratings are more easily detected than a single grating, in keeping with probability theory. Here we test the rules for the detection of 2-component compound gratings by extending the range of parameters used in previous experiments. Two complementary methods of deriving summation indices are described. Data are presented so that the conditions for the transition from probability to neural summation are easily identified. True probability summation occurs only when grating contrasts are carefully perceptually equalised and spatial frequency differs by more than a factor of 2. A wide range of contrast ratios of the component gratings were explored such that gratings were at different contrasts, relative to respective thresholds. We find clear evidence of suppressive interactions when the compound gratings are composed of a close to threshold low frequency component and a below-threshold higher spatial frequency component.


Subject(s)
Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Adult , Humans , Middle Aged , Motion Perception/physiology , Perceptual Masking , Photic Stimulation , Psychophysics , Sensory Thresholds
8.
Vis Neurosci ; 25(3): 327-31, 2008.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18598403

ABSTRACT

Using double silent substitution, it is possible to generate L-cone and M-cone isolating electroretinograms (ERGs) on a CRT. A major limitation of the technique is that the depth of modulation of cone classes is limited by the restricted luminance of the phosphors and their spectral overlap. To address this problem we have ported the technique to a four-color LED Ganzfeld stimulus (Diagnosis ColorDome). This allows higher retinal illuminances, higher contrasts, and triple silent substitution. With careful control over the retinal area stimulated, we show that the same data can be recorded from both CRT and LED stimuli when luminance, size and cone contrast are kept constant. Importantly, the different temporal profiles of the two devices do not influence the ERG amplitude and phase plots. We present data over a much wider range of luminances (up to about 10,000 trolands) and contrasts with the LED stimulator than previously reported with CRT screens. We conclude that the close resemblance between data obtained with an LED stimulator and with a CRT screen indicate that the differences have a purely physiological origin.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Electroretinography/methods , Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Retina/physiology , Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Adult , Contrast Sensitivity , Humans , Middle Aged , Photic Stimulation , Photoreceptor Cells/cytology , Retinal Rod Photoreceptor Cells/physiology
9.
Vis Neurosci ; 25(3): 405-10, 2008.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18598410

ABSTRACT

Simple reaction times (RTs) were measured to brief temporally blurred (total onset 570 ms) Gaussian isoluminant chromatic patches (s.d. 0.5 degrees) whose chromaticities lay along the cardinal chromatic axes (0 degrees, 90 degrees, 180 degrees, and 270 degrees in MBDKL color space). Bipolar adapting stimuli were employed (0 degrees versus 180 degrees or 90 degrees versus 270 degrees). These were larger Gaussian blobs (s.d. 1 degree), modulating sinusoidally between the two hues at 1 Hz. Throughout, the background was illuminant "C" (x = 0.31, y = 0.316, L = 12.5). In a single run, a series of 64 or 32 stimuli were presented without adaptation, followed by 64 or 32 stimuli each of which was preceded by 3 s of adaptation, either along the same or the orthogonal chromatic axis. Finally, 192 or 128 RTs were recorded to measure the time course of recovery from adaptation. Both adapting and test stimuli were presented at fixed supra-threshold contrasts. The effect of adaptation was seen as a lengthening of the RT, which occurred in the first few seconds of the adaptation period. After cessation of adaptation, there was a similarly rapid shortening of RT, although full recovery took 60-90 s. Adaptation gain functions suggested that the S-(L + M) system was less prone to adaptation than L-M.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Ocular/physiology , Color Perception/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Reaction Time , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Humans , Time Factors
10.
Vis Neurosci ; 23(3-4): 503-7, 2006.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16961987

ABSTRACT

Changes of color perception in the peripheral field are measured using an asymmetric simultaneous matching paradigm. The data confirm previous observations in that saturation changes can be neutralized if the test target is increased in size. However, this compensation does not apply to hue shifts. We show that some hues remain unchanged with eccentricity whereas others exhibit substantial changes. Here the color shifts are plotted in terms of a second-stage cone opponent model. The data suggest that the S-L+M channel is more robust to increasing eccentricity than the L-M channel. Observations are interpreted in terms of the known underlying morphological and physiological differences in these channels.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Retina/cytology , Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Visual Fields/physiology , Adaptation, Ocular/physiology , Adult , Attention/physiology , Color Perception Tests/methods , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation/methods , Retina/physiology
12.
Vis Neurosci ; 21(3): 231-5, 2004.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15518193

ABSTRACT

It is implicit in many cone-specific ERG studies that the amplitude is proportional to the numbers of cones stimulated. The objective of these experiments was to test this idea by comparing ERGs obtained from different areas of the retina with histological data on cone-density distributions. The histology (Curcio et al., 1990) shows that the cumulative number of cones in the human retina increases exponentially with stimulus diameter between 0- and 40-deg eccentricity. L-, M-, and (L+M) cone-driven 30-Hz ERGs were obtained from a series of stimuli with one of the following configurations: (1) Circular stimuli of different angular subtense up to 70-deg diameter. (2) Annuli with 70-deg outer diameter but variable inner diameter. (3) Annuli of constant area but increasing eccentricity. Cone contrasts were equalized for each stimulus condition. The modulated and nonmodulated regions of the screen had the same mean hue and luminance. The data suggest that the L+M cone ERG amplitude increases with stimulus diameter in direct proportion to the estimated number of cones stimulated. Furthermore, the total L+M responses appear to be predicted from individual L and M responses by simple linear summation for both the disc and annular stimuli.


Subject(s)
Electroretinography/methods , Photoreceptor Cells/anatomy & histology , Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells/anatomy & histology , Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Humans
13.
Vis Neurosci ; 21(3): 237-42, 2004.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15518194

ABSTRACT

To investigate the effect of foveal inhomogeneities on sensitivity to chromatic stimuli, we measured simple reaction times (RTs) and detection thresholds to temporally and spatially blurred isoluminant stimuli at retinal eccentricities from 0 deg to 8 deg. Three color-normal subjects participated. Contrast gain was derived from the slope of the RT versus contrast function. With a Gaussian spatial distribution (S.D. = 0.5 deg) and modulation between white (CIE x, y, L = 0.31, 0.316, 12.5 cd x m(-2)) and blue (MBDKL 90 deg), gain was maximal at about 2-deg eccentricity and declined by approximately 1 log unit towards the center and the periphery. The red (0 deg) and green (180 deg) cardinal axes showed maximum gain in the center, whilst the yellow (270 deg) data were intermediate. Although the spatial extent of the Gaussian spot was much larger than the S-cone free zone, we wished to determine whether foveal tritanopia was responsible for the marked drop in sensitivity to the 90-deg stimulus. To align the color vector along a tritan line, we used a smaller disk (0.3 deg) with a blurred edge and measured detection threshold, rotating the vector until minimum central sensitivity was obtained. Other workers have used transient tritanopia or minimally distinct border to similar effect. By repeating this at different locations in color space, a group of vectors were obtained. These converged near to the S-cone co-punctal point, evidence that they lay along tritan confusion lines. These threshold findings were then confirmed using the RT-derived contrast gain function. The tritan vectors were less pronounced as stimulus size increased. With the vector optimized to produce foveal tritanopia, the RT gain versus eccentricity functions for the 90-deg and 270-deg stimuli both fell markedly in the center and periphery, and sensitivity peaked at about 3-deg eccentricity. There are some similarities between these findings and the underlying photoreceptor distributions. As a result, there is a greater difference in gain between red-green and blue-yellow systems in the center than in the near periphery. We conclude that the RT versus contrast function is a sensitive index of foveal opponency.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Fovea Centralis/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Color Vision Defects/physiopathology , Color Vision Defects/psychology , Humans , Photic Stimulation , Reference Values , Reproducibility of Results
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...