Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 7 de 7
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 79(1): 49-65, 2000 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10909877

ABSTRACT

The unidimensional model of acculturation posits that heritage and mainstream culture identifications have a strong inverse relation, whereas the bidimensional model posits that the 2 identifications are independent. The authors compared these models in 3 samples of ethnic Chinese (ns = 164, 150, and 204), 1 sample of non-Chinese East Asians (n = 70), and one diverse group of acculturating individuals (n = 140). Although the unidimensional measure showed a coherent pattern of external correlates, the bidimensional measure revealed independent dimensions corresponding to heritage and mainstream culture identification. These dimensions displayed patterns of noninverse correlations with personality, self-identity, and psychosocial adjustment. The authors conclude that the bidimensional model is a more valid and useful operationalization of acculturation.


Subject(s)
Acculturation , Cultural Characteristics , Ethnicity/psychology , Social Adjustment , Social Identification , Adolescent , Adult , British Columbia , China/ethnology , Cross-Cultural Comparison , Asia, Eastern/ethnology , Female , Humans , Male , Models, Psychological , Surveys and Questionnaires
2.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 74(4): 1041-55, 1998 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9569658

ABSTRACT

Prior research (R.R. McCrae, P.T. Costa, & M.S. Yik, 1996) using a Chinese translation of the Revised NEO Personality Inventory suggested substantial differences between Hong Kong and North American undergraduates. Study 1, with a sample of bilingual Hong Kong students (N = 162), showed that prior findings were not due simply to the translation. Study 2, with undergraduates of European and Chinese ancestry living in Canada (N = 633), suggested that more of the differences were cultural in origin. Study 3, which used peer ratings of Chinese students (N = 99), replicated most Study 2 results, suggesting that exposure to Canadian culture increased openness, cheerfulness, and prosocial behavior and attitudes. Differences in sense of competence and vulnerability to stress appeared to be due to different cultural standards for judging these traits. Together, the 3 studies illustrate an integrated approach to interpreting personality differences across cultures.


Subject(s)
Acculturation , Cross-Cultural Comparison , Personality Inventory , Personality , Psychometrics , Adolescent , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Canada , China/ethnology , Female , Hong Kong , Humans , Male , Peer Group , Reproducibility of Results , Social Perception , Translating
3.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 74(5): 1197-208, 1998 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9599439

ABSTRACT

Reactions to trait self-enhancers were investigated in 2 longitudinal studies of person perception in discussion groups. Groups of 4-6 participants met 7 times for 20 min. After Meetings 1 and 7, group members rated their perceptions of one another. In Study 1, trait self-enhancement was indexed by measures of narcissism and self-deceptive enhancement. At the first meeting, self-enhancers made positive impressions. They were seen as agreeable, well adjusted, and competent. After 7 weeks, however, they were rated negatively and gave self-evaluations discrepant with peer evaluations they received. In Study 2, an independent sample of observers (close acquaintances) enabled a pretest index of discrepancy self-enhancement: It predicted the same deteriorating pattern of interpersonal perceptions as the other three trait measures. Nonetheless, all self-enhancement measures correlated positively with self-esteem.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Psychological , Attitude , Interpersonal Relations , Narcissism , Peer Group , Personality , Self Concept , Social Desirability , Adult , Deception , Female , Humans , Male , Personality Tests , Predictive Value of Tests , Time Factors
4.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 72(3): 581-91, 1997 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9120785

ABSTRACT

Perceptions of intelligence were investigated in 2 longitudinal studies of leaderless discussion groups (LDGs). In Study 1 (N = 87), students completed trait-shyness questionnaires and met 7 times in groups of 4-5. After Meetings 2 and 7, participants rated all group members on state shyness and intelligence. Trait-shy participants were initially judged to be less intelligent on both self- and peer ratings. At Time 2, however, trait-shy participants were no longer derogated by peers. Study 2 (N = 103) replicated the same pattern of shy derogation while demonstrating no actual relation between IQ and trait shyness. Again, trait-shy derogation disappeared by Time 2, but state-shy derogation continued. The state shy were now the low-IQ participants, who had begun to talk less. Thus, the bias against quiet individuals, originally inappropriate, gradually became a valid cue for low intelligence. Results were traced to overlapping cues for intelligence and shyness in LDGs.


Subject(s)
Intelligence , Leadership , Perception , Shyness , Female , Humans , Interpersonal Relations , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Self Concept , Surveys and Questionnaires
5.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 69(6): 1233-42, 1995 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8531058

ABSTRACT

D. A. Kenny (1994) estimated the components of personality rating variance to be 15, 20, and 20% for target, rater, and relationship, respectively. To enhance trait variance and minimize rater variance, we designed a series of studies of personality perception in discussion groups (N = 79, 58, and 59). After completing a Big Five questionnaire, participants met 7 times in small groups. After Meetings 1 and 7, group members rated each other. By applying the Social Relations Model (D. A. Kenny and L. La Voie, 1984) to each Big Five dimension at each point in time, we were able to evaluate 6 rating effects as well as rating validity. Among the findings were that (a) target variance was the largest component (almost 30%), whereas rater variance was small (less than 11%); (b) rating validity improved significantly with acquaintance, although target variance did not; and (c) no reciprocity was found, but projection was significant for Agreeableness.


Subject(s)
Interpersonal Relations , Personality , Social Perception , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Observer Variation , Personality Assessment/statistics & numerical data , Projection , Psychometrics , Self Concept
6.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 50(6): 1235-9, 1986 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3723338

ABSTRACT

It is known that in children, individual differences in temperament are linked to rates of minor physical anomalies (MPAs). These anomalies are a standard set of 17 nonobvious but measurable characteristics of the face, hands, and feet. We explored the relation between MPAs and temperament in adults. An intact sample of 88 undergraduates completed a battery of personality tests and lifestyle questions, and then were examined for MPAs. The index of MPAs for each subject was simply the total count out of 17. The results showed a pattern of correlates parallel to that found in children. Among male subjects, correlations with MPAs were significant for the Physical Activity and Clumsiness factors of the lifestyle inventory as well as for measures of emotionality, extraversion, masculinity, femininity, and Type A personality. The MPA index was also predictive of a behavioral index of temperament in male subjects. No correlations were significant among female subjects.


Subject(s)
Congenital Abnormalities/psychology , Personality Development , Personality , Temperament , Adult , Age Factors , Extraversion, Psychological , Female , Humans , Life Style , Male , Personality Tests , Sex Factors
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...