Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 24
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Exp Brain Res ; 233(12): 3467-74, 2015 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26298043

ABSTRACT

Making step adjustments is an essential component of walking. However, the ability to make step adjustments may be compromised when the walker's attentional capacity is limited. This study compared the effects of aging and dual tasking on step adjustments in response to stepping-target perturbations during visually cued treadmill walking. Fifteen older adults (69.4 ± 5.0 years; mean ± SD) and fifteen young adults (25.4 ± 3.0 years) walked at a speed of 3 km/h on a treadmill. Both groups performed visually cued step adjustments in response to unpredictable shifts of projected stepping targets in forward (FW), backward (BW) or sideward (SW) directions, at different levels of task difficulty [which increased as the available response distance (ARD) decreased], and with and without dual tasking (auditory Stroop task). In both groups, step adjustments were smaller than required. For FW and BW shifts, older adults undershot more under dual-task conditions. For these shifts, ARD affected the age groups differentially. For SW shifts, larger errors were found for older adults, dual tasking and the most difficult ARD. Stroop task performance did not differ between groups in all conditions. Older adults have more difficulty than young adults to make corrective step adjustments while walking, especially under dual-tasking conditions. Furthermore, they seemed to prioritize the cognitive task over the step adjustment task, a strategy that may pose aging populations at a greater fall risk. For comparable task difficulty, the older adults performed considerably worse than the young adults, indicating a decreased ability to adjust steps under time pressure.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Executive Function/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Walking/physiology , Adult , Aged , Auditory Perception/physiology , Cues , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Stroop Test , Young Adult
2.
Eur J Pain ; 19(7): 1021-34, 2015 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25690558

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Motor dysfunction in complex regional pain syndrome (CRPS) has been associated with bilateral malfunction of sensory and motor circuits, which hints at abnormal coupling between the affected and the contralateral unaffected limb. In addition, clinical observations suggest that motor performance may depend on the (voluntary or automatic) context in which movements are executed. The present study aimed to examine the role of voluntary and automatic aspects of interlimb coupling in CRPS. METHODS: Twenty patients with unilateral chronic CRPS and 40 healthy controls performed a set of unimanual and bimanual motor tasks that differed in the degree to which intended bilateral planning, intended afference-based error correction and unintended reflex-like entrainment were involved. RESULTS: Stability of interlimb coordination was reduced in CRPS patients compared to controls, especially for tasks involving active control of the affected side. In CRPS patients, intended coupling between the hands (planning, error correction) was markedly impaired, whereas unintended coupling between the hands (entrainment) appeared normal. CONCLUSIONS: Impaired motor control of the affected limb interfered with bimanual coordination, in particular for tasks involving voluntary (intended) as opposed to automatic (unintended) interlimb coupling. Our findings suggest inappropriate functioning of higher order centres involved in motor control of the affected limb, probably due to pain-related processes and impaired processing of proprioceptive information. Motor function of the affected limb may benefit from intended synchronization with movements of the unaffected contralateral limb, suggesting that bilateral training may be useful in patients with unilateral CRPS.


Subject(s)
Complex Regional Pain Syndromes/physiopathology , Upper Extremity/physiopathology , Complex Regional Pain Syndromes/complications , Complex Regional Pain Syndromes/psychology , Electromyography , Female , Functional Laterality , Hand/physiopathology , Humans , Kinesthesis , Male , Middle Aged , Movement Disorders/etiology , Movement Disorders/physiopathology , Muscle, Skeletal/innervation , Muscle, Skeletal/physiopathology , Pain Measurement , Proprioception , Psychomotor Performance , Reflex
3.
Motor Control ; 17(4): 399-411, 2013 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23756357

ABSTRACT

In the current study, we examined whether coupling influences resulting from unintended afference-based phase entrainment are affected by movement amplitude as such or by the amplitude relation between the limbs. We assessed entrainment strength by studying how passive movements of the contralateral hand influenced unimanual coordination with a metronome. Results showed that amplitude as such did not affect entrainment strength, whereas the amplitude relation between the hands did. Larger amplitudes of the passive hand relative to the active hand resulted in stronger entrainment. This dependence on relative amplitude implies that entrainment strength is not only based on the intensity of afferent signals generated in the entraining limb but also on the susceptibility of the entrained limb to these signals.


Subject(s)
Hand/physiology , Movement/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Electric Stimulation/methods , Female , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Wrist Joint/physiology , Young Adult
4.
Eur J Pain ; 17(2): 145-57, 2013 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22718534

ABSTRACT

Compelling evidence exists that pain may affect the motor system, but it is unclear if different sources of peripheral limb pain exert selective effects on motor control. This systematic review evaluates the effects of experimental (sub)cutaneous pain, joint pain, muscle pain and tendon pain on the motor system in healthy humans. The results show that pain affects many components of motor processing at various levels of the nervous system, but that the effects of pain are largely irrespective of its source. Pain is associated with inhibition of muscle activity in the (painful) agonist and its non-painful antagonists and synergists, especially at higher intensities of muscle contraction. Despite the influence of pain on muscle activation, only subtle alterations were found in movement kinetics and kinematics. The performance of various motor tasks mostly remained unimpaired, presumably as a result of a redistribution of muscle activity, both within the (painful) agonist and among muscles involved in the task. At the most basic level of motor control, cutaneous pain caused amplification of the nociceptive withdrawal reflex, whereas insufficient evidence was found for systematic modulation of other spinal reflexes. At higher levels of motor control, pain was associated with decreased corticospinal excitability. Collectively, the findings show that short-lasting experimentally induced limb pain may induce immediate changes at all levels of motor control, irrespective of the source of pain. These changes facilitate protective and compensatory motor behaviour, and are discussed with regard to pertinent models on the effects of pain on motor control.


Subject(s)
Movement/physiology , Pain/physiopathology , Pain/psychology , Biomechanical Phenomena , Brain/physiopathology , Efferent Pathways/physiopathology , Humans , Motor Neurons/physiology , Muscle, Skeletal/physiopathology , Pain Measurement , Proprioception/physiology , Reflex/physiology , Task Performance and Analysis
5.
Ann Biomed Eng ; 41(8): 1726-39, 2013 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23099793

ABSTRACT

Continuous rhythmic movements are often geared toward particular points in the movement cycle, as evidenced by a local reduction in trajectory variability. These so-called anchor points provide a window into motor control, since changes in the degree of anchoring may reveal how informational and/or neuromuscular properties are exploited in the organization of rhythmic movements. The present experiment examined the relative contributions of informational timing (metronome beeps) and neuromuscular (wrist postures) constraints on anchoring by systematically varying both factors at movement reversal points. To this end, participants cycled their right wrist in a flexed, neutral, or extended posture, either self-paced or synchronized to a metronome pacing peak flexion, peak extension, or both peak flexion and extension. The effects of these manipulations were assessed in terms of kinematics, auditory-motor coordination, and muscle activity. The degree of anchoring seen at the reversal points depended on the degree of compatibility of the prevailing configuration of neuromuscular and informational timing constraints, which had largely independent effects. We further observed systematic changes in muscular activity, which revealed distinct contributions of posture- and muscle-dependent neuromuscular properties to motor control. These findings indicate that the anchor-based discretization of the control of continuous rhythmic wrist movements is determined by both informational timing and neuromuscular constraints in a task-specific manner with subtle interactions between the two, and exemplify how movement variability may be exploited to gain such insights.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Physiological/physiology , Biological Clocks/physiology , Models, Neurological , Movement/physiology , Muscle Contraction/physiology , Muscle, Skeletal/physiology , Wrist Joint/physiology , Adult , Computer Simulation , Female , Humans , Male , Neuromuscular Junction/physiology , Periodicity , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Range of Motion, Articular/physiology , Young Adult
6.
Motor Control ; 17(2): 176-89, 2013 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23155092

ABSTRACT

Rhythmic limb movements are often anchored at particular points in the movement cycle. Anchoring may reveal essential task-specific information for motor control. We examined the effect of tracking mode (in-phase, antiphase) and gaze direction (left, right) on anchoring in visuomotor tracking with and without concurrent visual feedback of the hand movement. For in-phase tracking, anchoring was observed at the foveated reversal point whereas for antiphase tracking anchoring was observed at both reversals, suggesting the presence of two reference points instead of one. Anchoring at the foveated reversal reflected gaze anchoring (i.e., coalignment of hand and gaze) while anchoring at the nonfoveated reversal reflected visuomotor synchronization (i.e., the hand was steered to the nonfoveated reversal coincident with a target reversal at the point of gaze). We propose that the number and location of anchor points play a crucial role in the underlying control by providing reference values for error correction processes.


Subject(s)
Feedback, Sensory/physiology , Hand/physiology , Movement/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Periodicity , Young Adult
7.
Motor Control ; 16(4): 537-59, 2012 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23162066

ABSTRACT

Developmental changes in bimanual coordination were examined in four age groups: 6/7, 10/11, 14/15 years, and young adults. Temporal coupling was assessed through the stabilizing contributions of interlimb interactions related to planning, error correction, and reflexes during rhythmic wrist movements, by comparing various unimanual and bimanual tasks involving passive and active movements. Spatial coupling was assessed via bimanual line-circle drawing. With increasing age, temporal stability improved. Relative contributions of planning and reflex interactions to the achieved stability did not change, whereas error correction improved. In-phase and antiphase coordination developed at similar rates; implications of this result were discussed in terms of mirror-activity inhibition. Overall spatial drawing performance (circularity, variability, smoothness) improved with age, and spatial interference was smaller in adults than children. Whereas temporal coupling increased from 6/7 years to adulthood, spatial coupling changed mainly after 14/15 years. This difference in the development of temporal and spatial coupling corresponds to the anterior-posterior direction of corpus callosum myelination as reported in the literature.


Subject(s)
Adolescent Development/physiology , Child Development/physiology , Movement/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Aging/physiology , Child , Female , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Male
8.
J Rehabil Med ; 44(2): 106-17, 2012 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22266762

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To compare the effects of unilateral and bilateral training on upper limb function after stroke with regard to two key factors: severity of upper limb paresis and time of intervention post-stroke. DESIGN: Systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. METHODS: Two authors independently selected trials for inclusion, assessed the methodological quality and extracted data. Study outcomes were pooled by calculating the (standardized) mean difference ((S)MD). Sensitivity analyses for severity and time of intervention post-stroke were applied when possible. RESULTS: All 9 studies involving 452 patients showed homogeneity. In chronic patients with a mild upper limb paresis after stroke a marginally significant SMD for upper limb activity performance (SMD 0.34; 95% confidence interval): 0.04-0.63), and marginally significant MDs for perceived upper limb activity performance (amount of use: MD 0.42; 95% confidence interval: 0.09-0.76, and quality of movement: MD 0.45; 95% confidence interval: 0.12-0.78) were found in favour of unilateral training. All other MDs and SMDs were non-significant. CONCLUSION: Unilateral and bilateral training are similarly effective. However, intervention success may depend on severity of upper limb paresis and time of intervention post-stroke.


Subject(s)
Exercise Therapy/methods , Paresis/rehabilitation , Stroke Rehabilitation , Humans , Paresis/classification , Severity of Illness Index , Upper Extremity
9.
Exp Brain Res ; 209(2): 159-69, 2011 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21221956

ABSTRACT

Acoustic metronomes and visual targets have been used in rehabilitation practice to improve pathological gait. In addition, they may be instrumental in evaluating and training instantaneous gait adjustments. The aim of this study was to compare the efficacy of two cue types in inducing gait adjustments, viz. acoustic temporal cues in the form of metronome beeps and visual spatial cues in the form of projected stepping stones. Twenty healthy elderly (aged 63.2 ± 3.6 years) were recruited to walk on an instrumented treadmill at preferred speed and cadence, paced by either metronome beeps or projected stepping stones. Gait adaptations were induced using two manipulations: by perturbing the sequence of cues and by imposing switches from one cueing type to the other. Responses to these manipulations were quantified in terms of step-length and step-time adjustments, the percentage correction achieved over subsequent steps, and the number of steps required to restore the relation between gait and the beeps or stepping stones. The results showed that perturbations in a sequence of stepping stones were overcome faster than those in a sequence of metronome beeps. In switching trials, switching from metronome beeps to stepping stones was achieved faster than vice versa, indicating that gait was influenced more strongly by the stepping stones than the metronome beeps. Together these results revealed that, in healthy elderly, the stepping stones induced gait adjustments more effectively than did the metronome beeps. Potential implications for the use of metronome beeps and stepping stones in gait rehabilitation practice are discussed.


Subject(s)
Cues , Gait/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation , Aged , Analysis of Variance , Exercise Test , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Walking/physiology
10.
Exp Brain Res ; 196(4): 511-27, 2009 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19543722

ABSTRACT

Catching a ball involves a dynamic transformation of visual information about ball motion into motor commands for moving the hand to the right place at the right time. We previously formulated a neural model for this transformation to account for the consistent leftward movement biases observed in our catching experiments. According to the model, these biases arise within the representation of target motion as well as within the transformation from a gaze-centered to a body-centered movement command. Here, we examine the validity of the latter aspect of our model in a catching task involving gaze fixation. Gaze fixation should systematically influence biases in catching movements, because in the model movement commands are only generated in the direction perpendicular to the gaze direction. Twelve participants caught balls while gazing at a fixation point positioned either straight ahead or 14 degrees to the right. Four participants were excluded because they could not adequately maintain fixation. We again observed a consistent leftward movement bias, but the catching movements were unaffected by fixation direction. This result refutes our proposal that the leftward bias partly arises within the visuomotor transformation, and suggests instead that the bias predominantly arises within the early representation of target motion, specifically through an imbalance in the represented radial and azimuthal target motion.


Subject(s)
Arm/physiology , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Movement/physiology , Orientation/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Adult , Arm/innervation , Bias , Central Nervous System/physiology , Computer Simulation , Female , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Models, Neurological , Muscle, Skeletal/innervation , Muscle, Skeletal/physiology , Neuropsychological Tests , Photic Stimulation , Space Perception/physiology , Visual Fields/physiology , Young Adult
11.
J Mot Behav ; 39(5): 433-46, 2007 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17827119

ABSTRACT

The authors studied pattern stability and error correction during in-phase and antiphase 4-ball fountain juggling. To obtain ball trajectories, they made and digitized high-speed film recordings of 4 highly skilled participants juggling at 3 different heights (and thus different frequencies). From those ball trajectories, the authors determined and analyzed critical events (i.e., toss, zenith, catch, and toss onset) in terms of variability of point estimates of relative phase and temporal correlations. Contrary to common findings on basic instances of rhythmic interlimb coordination, in-phase and antiphase patterns were equally variable (i.e., stable). Consistent with previous findings, however, pattern stability decreased with increasing frequency. In contrast to previous results for 3-ball cascade juggling, negative lag-one correlations for catch-catch intervals were absent, but the authors obtained evidence for error corrections between catches and toss onsets. That finding may have reflected participants' high skill level, which yielded smaller errors that allowed for corrections later in the hand cycle.


Subject(s)
Motor Skills/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Adult , Algorithms , Biomechanical Phenomena , Data Interpretation, Statistical , Female , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Male
12.
Brain Res ; 1109(1): 60-9, 2006 Sep 13.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16860292

ABSTRACT

To gain insight into the brain activity associated with the performance of an acoustically paced synchronization task, we analyzed the amplitude and phase dynamics inherent in magnetoencephalographic (MEG) signals across frequency bands in order to discriminate between evoked and induced responses. MEG signals were averaged with respect to motor and auditory events (tap and tone onsets). Principal component analysis was used to compare amplitude and phase changes during listening and during paced and unpaced tapping, allowing a separation of brain activity related to motor and auditory processes, respectively. Motor performance was accompanied by phasic amplitude changes and increased phase locking in the beta band. Auditory processing of acoustic stimuli resulted in a simultaneous increase of amplitude and phase locking in the theta and alpha band. The temporal overlap of auditory-related amplitude changes and phase locking indicated an evoked response, in accordance with previous studies on auditory perception. The temporal difference of movement-related amplitude and phase dynamics in the beta band, on the other hand, suggested a change in ongoing brain activity, i.e., an induced response supporting previous results on motor-related brain dynamics in the beta band.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping , Brain/physiology , Evoked Potentials, Auditory/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Time Perception/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation/methods , Adult , Humans , Magnetoencephalography/methods , Male , Nonlinear Dynamics , Time Factors
13.
Hum Mov Sci ; 24(3): 379-402, 2005 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16087264

ABSTRACT

The effects of correct and transformed visual feedback on rhythmic unimanual visuo-motor tracking were examined, focusing on tracking performance (accuracy and stability) and visual search behavior. Twelve participants (reduced to 9 in the analyses) manually tracked an oscillating visual target signal in phase (by moving the hand in the same direction as the target signal) and in antiphase (by moving the hand in the opposite direction), while the frequency of the target signal was gradually increased to probe pattern stability. Besides a control condition without feedback, correct feedback (representing the actual hand movement) or mirrored feedback (representing the hand movement transformed by 180 degrees) were provided during tracking, resulting in either in-phase or antiphase visual motion of the target and feedback signal, depending on the tracking mode performed. The quality (accuracy and stability) of in-phase tracking was hardly affected by the two forms of feedback, whereas antiphase tracking clearly benefited from mirrored feedback but not from correct feedback. This finding extends previous results indicating that the performance of visuo-motor coordination tasks is aided by visual feedback manipulations resulting in coherently grouped (i.e., in-phase) visual motion structures. Further insights into visuo-motor tracking with and without feedback were garnered from the visual search patterns accompanying task performance. Smooth pursuit eye movements only occurred at lower oscillation frequencies and prevailed during in-phase tracking and when target and feedback signal moved in phase. At higher frequencies, point-of-gaze was fixated at a location that depended on the feedback provided and the resulting visual motion structures. During in-phase tracking the mirrored feedback was ignored, which explains why performance was not affected in this condition. Point-of-gaze fixations at one of the end-points were accompanied by reduced motor variability at this location, reflecting a form of visuo-motor anchoring that may support the pick up of discrete information as well as the control of hand movements to a desired location.


Subject(s)
Exploratory Behavior , Feedback , Motion Perception , Movement , Periodicity , Visual Perception , Adult , Female , Functional Laterality , Hand/physiology , Humans , Male , Movement/physiology
14.
J Mot Behav ; 37(3): 186-96, 2005 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15883116

ABSTRACT

On the basis of findings emphasizing the role of perceptual consequences in movement coordination, the authors tested the hypothesis that the learning of a new bimanual relative phase pattern would involve the matching of the movement-related sensory consequences (rather than the motor outflow commands) to the to-be-learned pattern. Two groups of participants (n = 10 in each) practiced rhythmically moving their forearms with a phase difference of 30 degrees . In 1 group, a difference in the arms' eigenfrequencies was imposed such that synchronous generation of the left and right motor commands resulted in the required relative phase (30 degrees ), yielding incongruence between the motor commands and their sensory consequences. In the other group, the experimenter imposed no eigenfrequency difference so that the sensory consequences were congruent with the motor commands. Throughout the practice period, performance of both groups was assessed repeatedly for the congruent situation (i.e., no eigenfrequency difference). On those criterion tests, both groups performed the required pattern equally well. The authors discuss that result, which corroborated the hypothesis, from a dynamical systems perspective.


Subject(s)
Feedback , Hand/physiology , Learning , Motor Skills/physiology , Movement , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Sensation , Adult , Algorithms , Female , Humans , Male , Practice, Psychological
15.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 358(1437): 1511-23, 2003 Sep 29.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14561342

ABSTRACT

In recent years, several phenomenological dynamical models have been formulated that describe how perceptual variables are incorporated in the control of motor variables. We call these short-route models as they do not address how perception-action patterns might be constrained by the dynamical properties of the sensory, neural and musculoskeletal subsystems of the human action system. As an alternative, we advocate a long-route modelling approach in which the dynamics of these subsystems are explicitly addressed and integrated to reproduce interceptive actions. The approach is exemplified through a discussion of a recently developed model for interceptive actions consisting of a neural network architecture for the online generation of motor outflow commands, based on time-to-contact information and information about the relative positions and velocities of hand and ball. This network is shown to be consistent with both behavioural and neurophysiological data. Finally, some problems are discussed with regard to the question of how the motor outflow commands (i.e. the intended movement) might be modulated in view of the musculoskeletal dynamics.


Subject(s)
Models, Biological , Motor Activity/physiology , Perception/physiology , Hand/physiology , Humans , Motor Neurons/physiology , Muscle, Skeletal/physiology , Time Factors
16.
J Mot Behav ; 35(1): 64-78, 2003 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12724100

ABSTRACT

The authors investigated how and to what extent visual information and associated task constraints are negotiated in the coordinative structure of playground swinging. Participants (N = 20) were invited to pump a swing from rest to a prescribed maximal amplitude under 4 conditions: normal vision, no vision, and 2 visual conditions involving explicit phasing constraints. In the latter conditions, participants were presented with a flow pattern consisting of a periodically expanding and contracting optical structure. They were instructed to phase the swing motion so that the forward turning point coincided with either the maximal size (enhanced optical flow) or the minimal size (reduced optical flow) of the presented flow pattern. Removal of visual information clearly influenced the swinging behavior, in that intersegmental coordination became more stereotyped, reflecting a general stiffening of the swinger. The conditions involving explicit phasing requirements also affected the coordination, but in an opposite way: The coordination became less stereotyped. The two phasing instructions had differential effects: The intersegmental coordination deviated more from normal swinging (i.e., without phasing constraints) when optical flow was enhanced than when it was reduced. Collectively, those findings show that visual information plays a formative role in the coordinative structure of swinging, in that variations of visual information and task constraints were accompanied by subtle yet noticeable changes in intersegmental coordination.


Subject(s)
Play and Playthings , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Models, Psychological , Spatial Behavior/physiology
17.
Brain Cogn ; 48(1): 149-65, 2002 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11812039

ABSTRACT

Although the Haken-Kelso-Bunz (HKB) model was originally formulated to account for phase transitions in bimanual movements, it evolved, through experimentation and conceptual elaboration, into a fundamental formal construct for the experimental study of rhythmically coordinated movements in general. The model consists of two levels of formalization: a potential defining the stability properties of relative phase and a system of coupled limit cycle oscillators defining the individual limb movements and their interactions. Whereas the empirical validity of the potential is well established, the validity of the formalization in terms of coupled oscillators is questionable, both with regard to the assumption that individual limb movements are limit cycle oscillators with (only) two active degrees of freedom and with regard to the postulated coupling. To remedy these limitations a more elaborate system of coupled oscillators is outlined, comprising two coupled limit cycle oscillators at the neural level, each of which is coupled to a linearly damped oscillator, representing the end-effectors.


Subject(s)
Extremities/physiology , Models, Psychological , Periodicity , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Humans
18.
J Mot Behav ; 32(4): 323-36, 2000 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11114226

ABSTRACT

Previous research on polyrhythmic performance can be broadly summarized in terms of 2 classes of models: timekeeper models and nonlinear dynamical models. In the former approach, research has been focused on patterns of covariance among time intervals, and in the latter approach, the concentration has been on pattern (in)stability and the spatiotemporal properties of oscillating limbs. It is suggested that one can achieve a more comprehensive theory that incorporates the strengths of each of these approaches by endowing timekeeper models with nonlinear dynamics or by endowing nonlinear oscillator models with stochastic variability. Additionally, those models are complemented by a topological description of performance based on knot theory. Knot theory provides a new index of difficulty for polyrhythmic tapping, a spatial interpretation of transitions between different stable rhythms, and a possible instantiation of N. A. Bernstein's (1967a) notion of a topological motor program.


Subject(s)
Attention , Motor Activity , Stereotyped Behavior , Stochastic Processes , Time Perception , Humans , Nonlinear Dynamics
19.
Biol Cybern ; 83(6): 529-42, 2000 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11130585

ABSTRACT

Schöner [Schöner G (1995) Ecol Psychol 7: 291-314] argued that the relative phase dynamics of rhythmic interlimb coordination may be attributed to the timing level in that the stability properties of the relative phase are largely independent of dynamical principles operating at the goal level, such as those related to the maintenance of a particular amplitude or target position. Yet, according to the coupling functions in the coupled oscillator model proposed by Haken et al. [Haken H, Kelso JAS, Bunz H (1985) Biol Cybern 51: 347-356], the effect of frequency on the stability properties of relative phase is either wholly or partially mediated by frequency-induced changes in amplitude, implying that the relative phase dynamics strongly depends on spatial factors. In order to distinguish between these contrasting interpretations of the organizational principles underwriting the phase dynamics of interlimb coordination, an experiment was conducted in which the effects of frequency and amplitude on the stability of relative phase were separated. Six subjects performed both in-phase and antiphase coordination patterns at seven different frequencies and three different amplitudes. Two measures of pattern stability were used, the standard deviation of relative phase and the exponent of the relaxation process following phasic perturbations of relative phase. According to both measures, pattern stability decreased with increasing frequency, whereas the amplitude manipulation only had a significant effect on the standard deviation of relative phase. This result was interpreted to imply that the organizational principles at the (relative) timing level are affected only moderately by task constraints pertaining to the goal level, and that models of interlimb coordination in which amplitude coupling plays a partial or subordinate role should be preferred above models relying solely on amplitude coupling.


Subject(s)
Elbow/physiology , Forearm/physiology , Models, Neurological , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Adult , Biomechanical Phenomena , Female , Functional Laterality , Humans , Muscle Contraction , Rotation
20.
Biol Cybern ; 83(5): 443-59, 2000 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11073208

ABSTRACT

Various stability features of bimanual rhythmic coordination, including phase transitions, have been modeled successfully by means of a one-dimensional equation of motion for relative phase obeying a gradient dynamics, the Haken-Kelso-Bunz model. The present study aimed at assessing pattern stability for stationary performance and estimating the model parameters (a, b, and Q) for the stochastic extension of this model. Estimates of a and b allowed for reconstruction of the potential defining the gradient dynamics. Two coordination patterns between the forearms (in-phase, antiphase) were performed at seven different frequencies. Model parameters were estimated on the basis of an exponential decay parameter describing the relaxation behavior of continuous relative phase following a mechanical perturbation. Variability of relative phase and relaxation time provided measures of pattern stability. Although the predicted inverse relation between pattern stability and movement frequency was observed for the lower tempo conditions, it was absent for the higher tempos, reflecting the influence of task constraints. No statistically significant differences in stability were observed between the two coordination modes, indicating the influence of intention. The reconstructed potential reflected the observed stability features, underscoring the adequacy of the parameter estimations. The relaxation process could not be captured adequately by means of a simple exponential decay function but required an additional oscillatory term. In accordance with previous assumptions, noise strength Q did not vary as a function of movement frequency. However, systematic differences in Q were observed between the two coordination modes. The advantages and (potential) pitfalls of using stationary performance of single patterns to examine the stability features of a bistable potential were discussed.


Subject(s)
Computer Simulation , Models, Biological , Models, Theoretical , Movement , Extremities , Humans
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...