Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 15 de 15
Filter
1.
Behav Res Methods ; 2023 Nov 28.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38017200

ABSTRACT

Attentional engagement is known to vary on a moment-to-moment basis. However, few self-report methods can effectively capture dynamic fluctuations in attentional engagement over time. In the current paper, we evaluated the utility of stimulated recall, a method wherein individuals are asked to remember their subjective states while using a mnemonic cue, for the measurement of temporal changes in attentional engagement. Participants were asked to watch a video lecture, during which we assessed their in-the-moment levels of attentional engagement using intermittent thought probes. Then, we used stimulated recall by cueing participants with short video clips from the lecture to retrospectively assess the levels of attentional engagement they had experienced when they first watched those clips within the lecture. Experiment 1 assessed the statistical overlap between in-the-moment and video-stimulated ratings. Experiment 2 assessed the generalizability of video-stimulated recall across different types of lectures. Experiment 3 assessed the impact of presenting video-stimulated probe clips in non-chronological order. Experiment 4 assessed the effect of video-stimulated recall on its own. Across all experiments, we found statistically robust correspondence between in-the-moment and video-stimulated ratings of attentional engagement, illustrating a strong convergence between these two methods of assessment. Taken together, our findings indicate that stimulated recall provides a new and practical methodological approach that can accurately capture dynamic fluctuations in subjective attentional states over time.

3.
Sci Rep ; 13(1): 781, 2023 01 16.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36646770

ABSTRACT

Interpretations of task performance in many cognitive studies rest on the assumption that participants are fully attentive to the tasks they agree to complete. However, with research studies being increasingly conducted online where monitoring participant engagement is difficult, this assumption may be inaccurate. If participants were found to be engaging in off-task behaviours while participating in these studies, the interpretation of study results might be called into question. To investigate this issue, we conducted a secondary data analysis across nearly 3000 participants in various online studies to examine the prevalence of one form of off-task behaviour: media multitasking. Rates of media multitasking were found to be high, averaging 38% and ranging from 9 to 85% across studies. Our findings broadly raise questions about the interpretability of results from online studies and urge researchers to consider the likelihood that participants are simultaneously engaging in off-task behaviours while completing online research tasks.


Subject(s)
Attention , Task Performance and Analysis , Humans , Probability , Prevalence
4.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 48(4): 289-311, 2022 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35298229

ABSTRACT

Humans spontaneously attend to faces and eyes. Recent findings, however, suggest that this social attentional biasing may not be driven by the social value of faces but by general factors, like stimulus content, visual context, or task settings. Here, we investigated whether the stimulus content factors of global luminance, featural configuration, and perceived attractiveness may independently drive social attentional biasing. Six experiments were run. In each, participants completed a dot-probe task where the presentation of a face, a house, and two neutral images was followed by the presentation of a response target at one of those locations. Experiments 1 and 2 assessed social attentional biasing when the face had higher overall global luminance. Experiments 3 and 4 assessed social attentional biasing when the face (but not the comparison house) retained the typical canonical configuration of internal features. Experiments 5 and 6 examined social attentional biasing when the face was more attractive than the house. Experiments 1, 3, and 5 measured manual responses when participants were instructed to maintain fixation. Experiments 2, 4, and 6 measured both manual and oculomotor responses when no instructions about eye movements were provided. The results indicated no reliable social attentional biasing in Experiments 1 to 5, however, a reliable saccadic bias toward the eyes of attractive upright faces was found in Experiment 6. Together, these results show that perceived facial attractiveness may be an important general factor in social attentional biasing. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Attentional Bias , Attention/physiology , Attentional Bias/physiology , Eye Movements , Humans
5.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 84(3): 829-842, 2022 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35084707

ABSTRACT

Although attention is thought to be spontaneously biased by social cues like faces and eyes, recent data have demonstrated that when extraneous content, context, and task factors are controlled, attentional biasing is abolished in manual responses while still occurring sparingly in oculomotor measures. Here, we investigated how social attentional biasing was affected by face novelty by measuring responses to frequently presented (i.e., those with lower novelty) and infrequently presented (i.e., those with higher novelty) face identities. Using a dot-probe task, participants viewed either the same face and house identity that was frequently presented on half of the trials or sixteen different face and house identities that were infrequently presented on the other half of the trials. A response target occurred with equal probability at the previous location of the eyes or mouth of the face or the top or bottom of the house. Experiment 1 measured manual responses to the target while participants maintained central fixation. Experiment 2 additionally measured participants' natural oculomotor behaviour when their eye movements were not restricted. Across both experiments, no evidence of social attentional biasing was found in manual data. However, in Experiment 2, there was a reliable oculomotor bias towards the eyes of infrequently presented upright faces. Together, these findings suggest that face novelty does not facilitate manual measures of social attention, but it appears to promote spontaneous oculomotor biasing towards the eyes of infrequently presented novel faces.


Subject(s)
Attentional Bias , Eye Movements , Attention/physiology , Attentional Bias/physiology , Cues , Face , Humans
6.
Front Psychol ; 11: 909, 2020.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32547441

ABSTRACT

Although mind wandering remains ubiquitous in daily life, the processes that underlie and sustain this behavior remain poorly understood. Across two experiments, we studied the role of intrinsic temperament traits, which shape stable behavioral processes, in moderating the association between mind wandering and the real-life functional outcome of academic success. In Experiment 1, participants completed the Mind Wandering Questionnaire, the Adult Temperament Questionnaire, and reported their grade for the highest degree completed or in progress. Individuals with traits of low Effortful control, high Negative affect, and low Extraversion indicated more mind wandering. Effortful control moderated the relationship between mind wandering and academic success, with higher tendency for mind wandering associated with higher academic achievement for individuals with high Effortful control, and lower academic achievement for those with low Effortful control. Experiment 2 confirmed these links using the visual metronome response task, an objective measure of mind wandering. Together, these results suggest that the intrinsic temperament trait of Effortful control represents one of the key mechanisms behind the functional influence of mind wandering on real-life outcomes. This work places an innate ability to control attention at the very core of real life success, and highlights the need for studying mind wandering through an interdisciplinary lens that brings together cognitive, biological, social, and clinical theories in order to understand the fundamental mechanisms that drive this behavior.

7.
Psychol Res ; 84(5): 1407-1423, 2020 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30603864

ABSTRACT

It is commonly accepted that attention is spontaneously biased towards faces and eyes. However, the role of stimulus features and task settings in this finding has not yet been systematically investigated. Here, we tested if faces and facial features bias attention spontaneously when stimulus factors, task properties, response conditions, and eye movements are controlled. In three experiments, participants viewed face, house, and control scrambled face-house images in an upright and inverted orientation. The task was to discriminate a target that appeared with equal probability at the previous location of the face, house, or the control image. In all experiments, our data indicated no spontaneous biasing of attention for targets occurring at the previous location of the face. Experiment 3, which measured oculomotor biasing, suggested a reliable but infrequent saccadic bias towards the eye region of upright faces. Importantly, these results did not reflect our specific laboratory settings, as in Experiment 4, we present a full replication of a classic finding in the literature demonstrating reliable social attention bias. Together, these data suggest that attentional biasing for social information is task and context mediated, and less robust than originally thought.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Attentional Bias/physiology , Orientation/physiology , Eye Movements/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
8.
Vision (Basel) ; 3(2)2019 Jun 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31735830

ABSTRACT

Humans spontaneously attend to social cues like faces and eyes. However, recent data show that this behavior is significantly weakened when visual content, such as luminance and configuration of internal features, as well as visual context, such as background and facial expression, are controlled. Here, we investigated attentional biasing elicited in response to information presented within appropriate background contexts. Using a dot-probe task, participants were presented with a face-house cue pair, with a person sitting in a room and a house positioned within a picture hanging on a wall. A response target occurred at the previous location of the eyes, mouth, top of the house, or bottom of the house. Experiment 1 measured covert attention by assessing manual responses while participants maintained central fixation. Experiment 2 measured overt attention by assessing eye movements using an eye tracker. The data from both experiments indicated no evidence of spontaneous attentional biasing towards faces or facial features in manual responses; however, an infrequent, though reliable, overt bias towards the eyes of faces emerged. Together, these findings suggest that contextually-based social information does not determine spontaneous social attentional biasing in manual measures, although it may act to facilitate oculomotor behavior.

9.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 26(4): 1273-1281, 2019 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31161527

ABSTRACT

Studies have established that scene context guides attention during visual search, but it is not yet clear how. In this study, we examined how attention is deployed across scenes using an attentional capture paradigm. Using the Surface Guidance Framework (Castelhano & Pereira, 2019), we defined target-relevant and target-irrelevant scene regions for each target object and compared how attentional capture of a suddenly onsetting distractor differs for object and letter searches. We found an enhancement of capture effects when distractors appeared within target-relevant regions, with greater proportions of distractors fixated and greater proportions of saccades made toward the distractor for object searches, but not for letter searches. Thus, attention in the real world can be flexibly and spatially distributed on the basis of contextual information, with the Surface Guidance Framework presenting a powerful tool for exploring attentional guidance in real-world scenes.


Subject(s)
Appetitive Behavior , Attention , Eye Movements , Space Perception , Visual Perception , Humans , Saccades
10.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 71(1): 229-240, 2018 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28429648

ABSTRACT

Many studies in reading have shown the enhancing effect of context on the processing of a word before it is directly fixated (parafoveal processing of words). Here, we examined whether scene context influences the parafoveal processing of objects and enhances the extraction of object information. Using a modified boundary paradigm called the Dot-Boundary paradigm, participants fixated on a suddenly onsetting cue before the preview object would onset 4° away. The preview object could be identical to the target, visually similar, visually dissimilar or a control (black rectangle). The preview changed to the target object once a saccade toward the object was made. Critically, the objects were presented on either a consistent or an inconsistent scene background. Results revealed that there was a greater processing benefit for consistent than inconsistent scene backgrounds and that identical and visually similar previews produced greater processing benefits than other previews. In the second experiment, we added an additional context condition in which the target location was inconsistent, but the scene semantics remained consistent. We found that changing the location of the target object disrupted the processing benefit derived from the consistent context. Most importantly, across both experiments, the effect of preview was not enhanced by scene context. Thus, preview information and scene context appear to independently boost the parafoveal processing of objects without any interaction from object-scene congruency.

11.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 44(2): 206-214, 2018 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28795836

ABSTRACT

Human social behavior is fine-tuned by interactions between individuals and their environments. Here we show that social motivation plays an important role in this process. Using a novel manipulation of social reward that included elements of real-life social exchanges, we demonstrate the emergence of attentional orienting for coincidental spatial associations that received positive social reward. After an interaction with the experimenter, participants completed a computerized task in which they received positive, negative, or no social reward for their performance to spatially congruent, spatially incongruent, and neutral cue-target pairings, respectively. Even though cue-target spatial correspondences remained at chance, attentional benefits emerged and persisted a day later for targets that received positive social reward. Our data further revealed that participants' level of social competence, as measured by the Autism-Spectrum Quotient scale, was predictably related to the magnitude of their reward-driven attentional benefits. No attentional effects emerged when the social interaction and social reward manipulations were removed. These results show that motivational incentives available during social exchanges affect later individual cognitive functioning, providing one of the first insights into why seemingly ambiguous social signals produce reliable and persistent attentional effects. (PsycINFO Database Record


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Interpersonal Relations , Motivation/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Reward , Social Behavior , Social Skills , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
12.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 40(5): 2056-72, 2014 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25089577

ABSTRACT

In the present study, we examined how gaze guidance is affected by immediately available information in the periphery and investigated how search strategies differed across manipulations in the availability of scene context and object content information. Across 3 experiments, participants performed a visual search task in scenes while using a gaze-contingent moving-window paradigm. Extrafoveal information was manipulated across conditions to examine the contributions of object content, scene context, or some combination of the two. Experiment 1 demonstrated a possible interaction between scene context and object content information in improving guidance. Experiments 2 and 3 supported the notion that object content is selected for further scrutiny based on its position within scene context. These results suggest a prioritization of object information based on scene context, such that contextual information acts as a framework in the selection of relevant regions, and object information can then affect which specific locations in those regions are selected for further examination.


Subject(s)
Eye Movements/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Fovea Centralis/physiology , Humans , Young Adult
13.
J Clin Sleep Med ; 9(12): 1259-66, 2013 Dec 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24340287

ABSTRACT

STUDY OBJECTIVES: Questionnaires have been validated as screening tools in adult populations at risk for obstructive sleep apnea (OSA). Portable monitors (PM) have gained acceptance for confirmation of OSA in some patients with a high pretest probability of the disorder. We evaluated the combined diagnostic utility of 3 validated questionnaires and a Level III PM in the diagnosis and exclusion of OSA, as compared with in-laboratory polysomnography (PSG) derived apnea hypopnea index (AHI). METHODS: Consecutive patients referred to the Sleep Disorders Clinic completed 3 testing components: (1) 3 questionnaires (Berlin, STOP-Bang, and Sleep Apnea Clinical Score [SACS]); (2) Level III at-home PM (MediByte) study; and (3) Level I in-laboratory PSG. The utility of individual questionnaires, the Level III device alone, and the combination of questionnaires and the Level III device were compared with the PSG. RESULTS: One hundred twenty-eight patients participated in the study (84M, 44F), mean ± SD age 50 ± 12.3years, BMI 31 ± 6.6 kg/m(2). At a PSG threshold AHI = 10, the PM derived respiratory disturbance index (RDI) had a sensitivity and specificity of 79% and 86%, respectively. The sensitivity and specificity for the other screening tools were: Berlin 88%, 25%; STOP-Bang 90%, 25%; SACS 33%, 75%. The sensitivity and specificity at a PSG AHI = 15 were: PM 77%, 95%; Berlin 91%, 28%; STOP-Bang 93%, 28%; SACS 35%, 78%. CONCLUSIONS: Questionnaires alone, possibly given a reliance on sleepiness as a symptom, cannot reliably rule out the presence of OSA. Objective physiological measurement is critical for the diagnosis and exclusion of OSA.


Subject(s)
Monitoring, Ambulatory/instrumentation , Monitoring, Ambulatory/methods , Polysomnography/instrumentation , Polysomnography/methods , Sleep Apnea, Obstructive/diagnosis , Surveys and Questionnaires , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Monitoring, Ambulatory/statistics & numerical data , Ontario , Polysomnography/statistics & numerical data , Prospective Studies , Reproducibility of Results , Sensitivity and Specificity
14.
Respir Physiol Neurobiol ; 183(3): 218-23, 2012 Sep 30.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22771782

ABSTRACT

Obstructive sleep apnoea (OSA) is associated with increased cardiovascular morbidity and mortality and hypercoagulability may be an underlying factor. We tested the hypotheses that patients with severe OSA are hypercoagulable and that two weeks of continuous positive airway pressure (CPAP) treatment reduces this hypercoagulability. In a prospective crossover study, twelve patients were randomized to either CPAP or no-CPAP for two weeks, a one week washout period, and then the other testing period for two weeks. Thromboelastography was used to assess coagulability at the start and end of each period and the apnoea-hypopnea indices (AHI) were measured at the end of each period. At baseline, ten patients had, compared to reference values, shorter clotting times, six increased rate of clot formation, twelve increased clot strength, and ten increased clotting indices. CPAP significantly reduced AHI (p=0.0003), clot strength (p=0.019) and clotting index (p=0.014). Hypercoagulability in patients with OSA can be detected by thromboelastography, and is reduced by CPAP.


Subject(s)
Continuous Positive Airway Pressure/methods , Severity of Illness Index , Sleep Apnea, Obstructive/therapy , Thrombelastography/methods , Thrombophilia/therapy , Adult , Cross-Over Studies , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Prospective Studies , Sleep Apnea, Obstructive/diagnosis , Sleep Apnea, Obstructive/physiopathology , Thrombophilia/diagnosis , Thrombophilia/physiopathology , Treatment Outcome
15.
Can Respir J ; 18(3): 137-43, 2011.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21766076

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Portable monitors are increasingly being used as a diagnostic screening tool for obstructive sleep apnea (OSA), and in-laboratory validation of these devices with polysomnography (PSG) is required. OBJECTIVE: To assess the reliability of the MediByte (Braebon Medical Corporation, Canada) type 3 screening device compared with overnight PSG. METHODS: To cover a range of OSA severity, a consecutive series of patients wore the screening device while simultaneously undergoing PSG. Data acquired from the screener and PSG were blinded and scored separately. The number of apneas and hypopneas per hour were calculated using recording time (respiratory disturbance index [RDI]) for the MediByte device, and sleep time (apnea-hypopnea index [AHI]) for PSG. RESULTS: Data from 73 patients with a mean age of 53 years and body mass index of 32.2 kg/m2 showed high measurement association between the RDI and AHI, with a Pearson correlation of 0.92, accounting for 85% of the variance. Based on Bland-Altman measurement agreement, the mean difference between the RDI and AHI (-5.9±11.2 events/h) indicated screener under-reporting. For an AHI of greater than 15 events/h, the sensitivity and specificity of the screener was 80% and 97%, respectively; for an AHI of greater than 30 events/h, the positive predictive value was 100%, while the negative predictive value was 88%. CONCLUSION: The MediByte device accurately identified patients without OSA and had a high sensitivity for moderate-to-severe OSA.


Subject(s)
Mass Screening , Polysomnography , Remote Sensing Technology , Sleep Apnea, Obstructive/diagnosis , Adult , Equipment Design/instrumentation , Equipment Design/methods , Female , Humans , Male , Mass Screening/instrumentation , Mass Screening/methods , Middle Aged , Polysomnography/instrumentation , Polysomnography/methods , Remote Sensing Technology/instrumentation , Remote Sensing Technology/methods , Reproducibility of Results , Sensitivity and Specificity , Sleep , Sleep Apnea, Obstructive/prevention & control , Time Factors
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...