Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 26
Filter
1.
Int J Obes (Lond) ; 38(3): 404-10, 2014 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23828101

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: Short sleep duration and sleep problems increase risks of overweight and weight gain. Few previous studies have examined sleep and weight repeatedly over development. This study examined the associations between yearly reports of sleep problems and weight status from ages 5 to 11. Although, previous studies have shown that inter-individual differences moderate the effect of short sleep duration on weight, it is not known whether inter-individual differences also moderate the effect of sleep problems on weight. We tested how the longitudinal associations between sleep problems and weight status were moderated by impulsivity and genetic variants in DRD2 and ANKK1. DESIGN: Seven-year longitudinal study. PARTICIPANTS: A total of 567 children from the Child Development Project for the analysis with impulsivity and 363 for the analysis with genetic variants. MEASUREMENTS AND RESULTS: Sleep problems and weight status were measured by mothers' reports yearly. Impulsivity was measured by teachers' reports yearly. Six single-nucleotide polymorphisms located in DRD2 and ANKK1 were genotyped. Data were analyzed using multilevel modeling. Higher average levels of sleep deprivation across years were associated with greater increases in overweight (P=0.0024). Sleep problems and overweight were associated at both within-person across time (P<0.0001) and between-person levels (P<0.0001). Impulsivity and two polymorphisms, rs1799978 and rs4245149 in DRD2, moderated the association between sleep problems and overweight; the association was stronger in children who were more impulsive (P=0.0022), in G allele carriers for rs1799978 (P=0.0007) and in A allele carriers for rs4245149 (P=0.0002). CONCLUSIONS: This study provided incremental evidence for the influence of sleep problems on weight. Findings of DRD2, ANKK1 and impulsivity are novel; they suggest that reward sensitivity and self-regulatory abilities might modulate the influences of sleep on weight gain. The analysis of polymorphisms was restricted to European Americans and hence the results might not generalize to other populations.


Subject(s)
Impulsive Behavior , Overweight/genetics , Polymorphism, Single Nucleotide , Protein Serine-Threonine Kinases/genetics , Receptors, Dopamine D2/genetics , Sleep Deprivation/genetics , Weight Gain , Alleles , Child , Child, Preschool , Female , Genotype , Humans , Linkage Disequilibrium , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Motor Activity , Overweight/etiology , Overweight/prevention & control , Sleep Deprivation/complications , Socioeconomic Factors , United States/epidemiology , White People
2.
Dev Psychopathol ; 13(2): 337-54, 2001.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11393650

ABSTRACT

A longitudinal, prospective design was used to examine the roles of peer rejection in middle childhood and antisocial peer involvement in early adolescence in the development of adolescent externalizing behavior problems. Both early starter and late starter pathways were considered. Classroom sociometric interviews from ages 6 through 9 years, adolescent reports of peers' behavior at age 13 years, and parent, teacher, and adolescent self-reports of externalizing behavior problems from age 5 through 14 years were available for 400 adolescents. Results indicate that experiencing peer rejection in elementary school and greater involvement with antisocial peers in early adolescence are correlated but that these peer relationship experiences may represent two different pathways to adolescent externalizing behavior problems. Peer rejection experiences, but not involvement with antisocial peers. predict later externalizing behavior problems when controlling for stability in externalizing behavior. Externalizing problems were most common when rejection was experienced repeatedly. Early externalizing problems did not appear to moderate the relation between peer rejection and later problem behavior. Discussion highlights multiple pathways connecting externalizing behavior problems from early childhood through adolescence with peer relationship experiences in middle childhood and early adolescence.


Subject(s)
Adolescent Behavior/psychology , Antisocial Personality Disorder/psychology , Child Behavior Disorders/psychology , Peer Group , Rejection, Psychology , Adolescent , Child , Child Behavior Disorders/diagnosis , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Interpersonal Relations , Male
3.
Child Dev ; 72(2): 583-98, 2001.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11333086

ABSTRACT

The early childhood antecedents and behavior-problem correlates of monitoring and psychological control were examined in this prospective, longitudinal, multi-informant study. Parenting data were collected during home visit interviews with 440 mothers and their 13-year-old children. Behavior problems (anxiety/depression and delinquent behavior) were assessed via mother, teacher, and/or adolescent reports at ages 8 through 10 years and again at ages 13 through 14. Home-interview data collected at age 5 years were used to measure antecedent parenting (harsh/reactive, positive/proactive), family background (e.g., socioeconomic status), and mother-rated child behavior problems. Consistent with expectation, monitoring was anteceded by a proactive parenting style and by advantageous family-ecological characteristics, and psychological control was anteceded by harsh parenting and by mothers' earlier reports of child externalizing problems. Consistent with prior research, monitoring was associated with fewer delinquent behavior problems. Links between psychological control and adjustment were more complex: High levels of psychological control were associated with more delinquent problems for girls and for teens who were low in preadolescent delinquent problems, and with more anxiety/depression for girls and for teens who were high in preadolescent anxiety/depression.


Subject(s)
Adolescent Behavior/psychology , Mother-Child Relations , Parenting/psychology , Social Adjustment , Adolescent , Anxiety/psychology , Child , Depression/psychology , Female , Humans , Internal-External Control , Juvenile Delinquency/psychology , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Prospective Studies
4.
J Fam Psychol ; 14(3): 380-400, 2000 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11025931

ABSTRACT

Direct and indirect precursors to parents' harsh discipline responses to hypothetical vignettes about child misbehavior were studied with data from 978 parents (59% mothers; 82% European American and 16% African American) of 585 kindergarten-aged children. SEM analyses showed that parents' beliefs about spanking and child aggression and family stress mediated a negative relation between socioeconomic status and discipline. In turn, perception of the child and cognitive-emotional processes (hostile attributions, emotional upset, worry about child's future, available alternative disciplinary strategies, and available preventive strategies) mediated the effect of stress on discipline. Similar relations between ethnicity and discipline were found (African Americans reported harsher discipline), especially among low-income parents. Societally based experiences may lead some parents to rely on accessible and coherent goals in their discipline, whereas others are more reactive.


Subject(s)
Black or African American/psychology , Internal-External Control , Parenting/psychology , Socialization , Socioeconomic Factors , Stress, Psychological/complications , White People/psychology , Child , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Male , Social Values
5.
J Abnorm Child Psychol ; 28(2): 161-79, 2000 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10834768

ABSTRACT

In a sample of 405 children assessed in kindergarten through the seventh grade, we determined the basic developmental trajectories of mother-reported and teacher-reported externalizing and internalizing behaviors using cross-domain latent growth modeling techniques. We also investigated the effects of race, socioeconomic level, gender, and sociometric peer-rejection status in kindergarten on these trajectories. The results indicated that, on average, the development of these behaviors was different depending upon the source of the data. We found evidence of the codevelopment of externalizing and internalizing behaviors within and across reporters. In addition, we found that African-American children had lower levels of externalizing behavior in kindergarten as reported by mothers than did European-American children but they had greater increases in these behaviors when reported by teachers. Children from homes with lower SES levels had higher initial levels of externalizing behaviors and teacher-reported internalizing behaviors. Males showed greater increases in teacher-reported externalizing behavior over time than did the females. Rejected children had trajectories of mother-reported externalizing and internalizing behavior that began at higher levels and either remained stable or increased more rapidly than did the trajectories for non-rejected children which decreased over time.


Subject(s)
Aggression , Child Behavior Disorders/psychology , Interpersonal Relations , Personality Development , Black or African American/psychology , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Male , Models, Psychological , Peer Group , Sex Factors , Social Desirability , Socioeconomic Factors , White People/psychology
6.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 77(2): 387-401, 1999 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10474213

ABSTRACT

In an 8-year prospective study of 173 girls and their families, the authors tested predictions from J. Belsky, L. Steinberg, and P. Draper's (1991) evolutionary model of individual differences in pubertal timing. This model suggests that more negative-coercive (or less positive-harmonious) family relationships in early childhood provoke earlier reproductive development in adolescence. Consistent with the model, fathers' presence in the home, more time spent by fathers in child care, greater supportiveness in the parental dyad, more father-daughter affection, and more mother-daughter affection, as assessed prior to kindergarten, each predicted later pubertal timing by daughters in 7th grade. The positive dimension of family relationships, rather than the negative dimension, accounted for these relations. In total, the quality of fathers' investment in the family emerged as the most important feature of the proximal family environment relative to daughters' pubertal timing.


Subject(s)
Family Relations , Family/psychology , Puberty/physiology , Adolescent , Age Factors , Child , Female , Humans , Longitudinal Studies , Parent-Child Relations , Prospective Studies , Time Factors
7.
J Abnorm Child Psychol ; 27(3): 191-201, 1999 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10438185

ABSTRACT

This study is a prospective investigation of the predictive association between early behavior problems (internalizing, externalizing, hyperactivity-impulsiveness, immaturity-dependency) and later victimization in the peer group. Teacher ratings of the behavioral adjustment of 389 kindergarten and 1st-grade children (approximate age range of 5 to 6 years-old) were obtained, using standardized behavior problem checklists. These ratings predicted peer nomination scores for victimization, obtained 3 years later, even after the prediction associated with concurrent behavior problems was statistically controlled. Further analyses suggested that the relation between early behavior problems and later victimization is mediated by peer rejection and moderated by children's dyadic friendships. Behavior problems appear to play an important role in determining victimization within the peer group, although the relevant pathways are complex and influenced by other aspects of children's social adjustment.


Subject(s)
Aggression , Child Behavior/psychology , Crime Victims/psychology , Social Behavior , Child , Child, Preschool , Female , Forecasting , Humans , Male , Peer Group , Prospective Studies , Risk Factors
8.
Child Dev ; 70(4): 896-909, 1999.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10446725

ABSTRACT

This study examined relations among mothers' hostile attribution tendencies regarding their children's ambiguous problem behaviors, mothers' harsh discipline practices, and children's externalizing behavior problems. A community sample of 277 families (19% minority representation) living in three geographic regions of the United States was followed for over 4 years. Mothers' hostile attribution tendencies were assessed during the summer prior to children's entry into kindergarten through their responses to written vignettes. Mothers' harsh discipline practices were assessed concurrently through ratings by interviewers and reports by spouses. Children's externalizing behavior problems were assessed concurrently through written questionnaires by mothers and fathers and in the spring of kindergarten and first, second, and third grades through reports by teachers and peer sociometric nominations. Results of structural equations models demonstrated that mothers' hostile attribution tendencies predicted children's future externalizing behavior problems at school and that a large proportion of this relation was mediated by mothers' harsh discipline practices. These results remained virtually unchanged when controlling for initial levels of children's prekindergarten externalizing behavior problems at home.


Subject(s)
Child Behavior Disorders/psychology , Hostility , Maternal Behavior/psychology , Mother-Child Relations , Mothers/psychology , Parenting , Child , Child, Preschool , Female , Follow-Up Studies , Humans , Internal-External Control , Interpersonal Relations , Male
9.
Child Dev ; 70(3): 768-78, 1999.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10368921

ABSTRACT

Unsupervised peer contact in the after-school hours was examined as a risk factor in the development of externalizing problems in a longitudinal sample of early adolescents. Parental monitoring, neighborhood safety, and adolescents' preexisting behavioral problems were considered as possible moderators of the risk relation. Interviews with mothers provided information on monitoring, neighborhood safety, and demographics. Early adolescent (ages 12-13 years) after-school time use was assessed via a telephone interview in grade 6 (N = 438); amount of time spent with peers when no adult was present was tabulated. Teacher ratings of externalizing behavior problems were collected in grades 6 and 7. Unsupervised peer contact, lack of neighborhood safety, and low monitoring incrementally predicted grade 7 externalizing problems, after controlling for family background factors and grade 6 problems. The greatest risk was for those unsupervised adolescents living in low-monitoring homes and comparatively unsafe neighborhoods. The significant relation between unsupervised peer contact and problem behavior in grade 7 held only for those adolescents who already were high in problem behavior in grade 6. These findings point to the need to consider individual, family, and neighborhood factors in evaluating risks associated with young adolescents' after-school care experiences.


Subject(s)
Adolescent Behavior , Leisure Activities/psychology , Parenting , Peer Group , Social Adjustment , Social Environment , Adolescent , Adult , Child , Child Behavior Disorders/epidemiology , Child Development , Child, Preschool , Disease Progression , Female , Follow-Up Studies , Humans , Indiana/epidemiology , Male , Multivariate Analysis , Regression Analysis , Residence Characteristics/statistics & numerical data , Safety/statistics & numerical data , Socialization , Tennessee/epidemiology
10.
Dev Psychol ; 34(5): 982-95, 1998 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9779744

ABSTRACT

Child temperament and parental control were studied as interacting predictors of behavior outcomes in 2 longitudinal samples. In Sample 1, data were ratings of resistant temperament and observed restrictive control in infancy-toddlerhood and ratings of externalizing behavior at ages 7 to 10 years; in Sample 2, data were retrospective ratings of temperament in infancy-toddlerhood, observed restrictive control at age 5 years, and ratings of externalizing behavior at ages 7 to 11 years. Resistance more strongly related to externalizing in low-restriction groups than in high-restriction groups. This was true in both samples and for both teacher- and mother-rated outcomes. Several Temperament x Environment interaction effects have been reported previously, but this is one of very few replicated effects.


Subject(s)
Child Behavior Disorders/psychology , Defense Mechanisms , Internal-External Control , Parenting/psychology , Personality Development , Temperament , Child , Child Behavior Disorders/diagnosis , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Infant , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Personality Assessment , Socialization
11.
Dev Psychopathol ; 10(3): 469-93, 1998.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9741678

ABSTRACT

The aim of this study was to test whether individual risk factors as well as the number of risk factors (cumulative risk) predicted children's externalizing behaviors over middle childhood. A sample of 466 European American and 100 African American boys and girls from a broad range of socioeconomic levels was followed from age 5 to 10 years. Twenty risk variables from four domains (child, sociocultural, parenting, and peer-related) were measured using in-home interviews at the beginning of the study, and annual assessments of externalizing behaviors were conducted. Consistent with past research, individual differences in externalizing behavior problems were stable over time and were related to individual risk factors as well as the number of risk factors present. Particular risks accounted for 36% to 45% of the variance, and the number of risks present (cumulative risk status) accounted for 19% to 32% of the variance, in externalizing outcomes. Cumulative risk was related to subsequent externalizing even after initial levels of externalizing had been statistically controlled. All four domains of risk variables made significant unique contributions to this statistical prediction, and there were multiple clusters of risks that led to similar outcomes. There was also evidence that this prediction was moderated by ethnic group status, most of the prediction of externalizing being found for European American children. However, this moderation effect varied depending on the predictor and outcome variables included in the model.


Subject(s)
Mental Disorders/epidemiology , Black or African American , Black People , Caregivers , Child , Child, Preschool , Cohort Studies , Culture , Ethnicity , Europe/ethnology , Female , Humans , Indiana/epidemiology , Interpersonal Relations , Interviews as Topic , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Nuclear Family , Parent-Child Relations , Risk Factors , Single Parent , Socioeconomic Factors , Tennessee/epidemiology , Urban Population , White People
12.
Dev Psychopathol ; 10(1): 87-99, 1998.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9524809

ABSTRACT

This study reports a short-term prospective investigation of the role of peer group victimization in the development of children's behavior problems, at home and in school. Sociometric interviews were utilized to assess aggression, victimization by peers, and peer rejection, for 330 children who were in either the third or fourth grade (approximate mean ages of 8-9 years old). Behavior problems were assessed using standardized behavior checklists completed by mothers and teachers. A follow-up assessment of behavior problems was completed 2 years later, when the children were in either the fifth or sixth grade (approximate mean ages of 10-11 years old). Victimization was both concurrently and prospectively associated with externalizing, attention dysregulation, and immature/dependent behavior. Victimization also predicted increases in these difficulties over time, and incremented the prediction in later behavior problems associated with peer rejection and aggression. The results of this investigation demonstrate that victimization in the peer group is an important predictor of later behavioral maladjustment.


Subject(s)
Child Behavior Disorders/diagnosis , Dominance-Subordination , Peer Group , Social Environment , Aggression/psychology , Attention Deficit and Disruptive Behavior Disorders/diagnosis , Attention Deficit and Disruptive Behavior Disorders/psychology , Child , Child Behavior Disorders/psychology , Follow-Up Studies , Humans , Internal-External Control , Personality Assessment , Prospective Studies , Rejection, Psychology , Social Adjustment
13.
Child Dev ; 68(4): 665-75, 1997 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9306645

ABSTRACT

This study reports the first prospective investigation of the early family experiences of boys who later emerged as both aggressive and bullied (i.e., aggressive victims) during their middle childhood years. It was hypothesized that a history of violent victimization by adults leads to emotion dysregulation that results in a dual pattern of aggressive behavior and victimization by peers. Interviews with mothers of 198 5-year-old boys assessed preschool home environments. Four to 5 years later, aggressive behavior and peer victimization were assessed in the school classroom. The early experiences of 16 aggressive victims were contrasted with those of 21 passive (nonaggressive) victims, 33 nonvictimized aggressors, and 128 normative boys. Analyses indicated that the aggressive victim group had experienced more punitive, hostile, and abusive family treatment than the other groups. In contrast, the nonvictimized aggressive group had a history of greater exposure to adult aggression and conflict, but not victimization by adults, than did the normative group, whereas the passive victim group did not differ from the normative group on any home environment variable.


Subject(s)
Aggression/psychology , Child Behavior , Peer Group , Psychology, Child , Social Behavior , Socialization , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Child , Humans , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Mothers/psychology , Surveys and Questionnaires
14.
Psychiatr Clin North Am ; 20(2): 283-99, 1997 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9196915

ABSTRACT

This article presents an overview of current research and thinking on the developmental course of aggression and violent behavior from early childhood through adolescence. Differing developmental trajectories are highlighted, and salient family and peer influences are described. Acquired social information processing patterns are discussed as potential mediators of the link between social experience in the family and peer group and individual differences in children's aggressive behavior. The need for developmentally sensitive models that focus on the interplay of family and peer experience, information processing patterns, and onset and chronicity of aggression is stressed.


Subject(s)
Aggression , Family/psychology , Peer Group , Violence , Female , Humans , Male , Parent-Child Relations
15.
Child Dev ; 68(2): 312-32, 1997 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9180004

ABSTRACT

Contributions of mothers' social coaching and responsive style to preschoolers' peer competence were evaluated in 2 studies. In Study 1, 43 mother-child dyads participated in 3 laboratory tasks; videotapes were coded for responsive interaction style in play, advice regarding videotaped peer dilemmas (coaching), and nonsocial teaching in a puzzle task. Coaching and style were largely independent and were correlated with measures of social competence. In Study 2 (n = 62), coaching and style uniquely predicted teacher ratings, but only style predicted peer acceptance. To investigate whether coaching mediated the effects of style and/or whether style moderated the effects of coaching, the samples were combined. No evidence was found for mediation, but coaching was a more powerful predictor of lower levels of boys' aggression when the mother-child relationship was less responsive. Discussion focuses on models of socialization that stress the interplay of general style and specific socialization practices in promoting social competence.


Subject(s)
Mother-Child Relations , Peer Group , Social Adjustment , Social Behavior , Socialization , Aggression/psychology , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Male , Maternal Behavior , Personality Assessment
16.
Child Dev ; 68(2): 278-94, 1997 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9180002

ABSTRACT

From a sample of 567 kindergartners observed during free play, 150 children were classified as socially withdrawn and followed over 4 years. A cluster analysis involving teacher ratings was used to identify subtypes of withdrawn children. Four clusters were identified, 3 fitting profiles found in the literature and labeled unsociable (n = 96), passive-anxious (n = 23), and active-isolate (n = 19), and 1 typically not discussed, labeled sad/depressed (n = 12). Sociometric ratings indicated that unsociable children had elevated rates of sociometric neglect, active-isolates had higher than expected levels of rejection, and sad/depressed children had elevated rates of both neglect and rejection. Subtypes also differed in social information-processing patterns, with active-isolate children displaying the least component skills. The findings that some experience more difficulty than others might account for the ambiguity in extant studies regarding whether or not social withdrawal is a risk factor in psychosocial development, because withdrawal has most often been treated as a unitary construct in the past.


Subject(s)
Cognition Disorders/diagnosis , Shyness , Social Behavior Disorders/diagnosis , Social Isolation , Sociometric Techniques , Anxiety/diagnosis , Anxiety/psychology , Child , Child, Preschool , Cognition Disorders/psychology , Cohort Studies , Depression/diagnosis , Depression/psychology , Female , Humans , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Personality Assessment , Rejection, Psychology , Social Behavior Disorders/psychology
17.
J Abnorm Psychol ; 106(1): 37-51, 1997 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9103716

ABSTRACT

The authors proposed that reactively aggressive and proactively aggressive types of antisocial youth would differ in developmental histories, concurrent adjustment, and social information-processing patterns. In Study 1, 585 boys and girls classified into groups called reactive aggressive, proactive aggressive, pervasively aggressive (combined type), and nonaggressive revealed distinct profiles. Only the reactive aggressive groups demonstrated histories of physical abuse and early onset of problems, adjustment problems in peer relations, and inadequate encoding and problem-solving processing patterns. Only the proactive aggressive groups demonstrated a processing pattern of anticipating positive outcomes for aggressing. In Study 2, 50 psychiatrically impaired chronically violent boys classified as reactively violent or proactively violent demonstrated differences in age of onset of problem behavior, adjustment problems, and processing problems.


Subject(s)
Aggression/classification , Child Development , Interpersonal Relations , Mental Disorders/complications , Self Concept , Adolescent , Analysis of Variance , Chi-Square Distribution , Child , Cohort Studies , Cross-Sectional Studies , Discriminant Analysis , Female , Humans , Male , Violence/classification
18.
Child Dev ; 67(5): 2417-33, 1996 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9022248

ABSTRACT

The present study focused on mother-child interaction predictors of initial levels and change in child aggressive and disruptive behavior at school from kindergarten to third grade. Aggression-disruption was measured via annual reports from teachers and peers. Ordinary least-squares regression was used to identify 8 separate child aggression trajectories, 4 for each gender: high initial levels with increases in aggression, high initial levels with decrease in aggression, low initial levels with increases in aggression, and low initial levels with decreases in aggression. Mother-child interaction measures of coercion and nonaffection collected prior to kindergarten were predictive of initial levels of aggression-disruption in kindergarten in both boys and girls. However, boys and girls differed in how coercion and nonaffection predicted change in aggression-disruption across elementary school years. For boys, high coercion and nonaffection were particularly associated with the high-increasing-aggression trajectory, but for girls, high levels of coercion and nonaffection were associated with the high-decreasing-aggression trajectory. This difference is discussed in the context of Patterson et al.'s coercion training theory, and the need for gender-specific theories of aggressive development is noted.


Subject(s)
Affect , Aggression/psychology , Child Behavior Disorders/psychology , Coercion , Gender Identity , Mother-Child Relations , Personality Development , Child , Child Behavior Disorders/diagnosis , Child, Preschool , Female , Follow-Up Studies , Humans , Male , Parenting/psychology , Socialization
19.
J Abnorm Psychol ; 104(4): 632-43, 1995 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8530766

ABSTRACT

The authors tested the hypothesis that early physical abuse is associated with later externalizing behavior outcomes and that this relation is mediated by the intervening development of biased social information-processing patterns. They assessed 584 randomly selected boys and girls from European American and African American backgrounds for the lifetime experience of physical abuse through clinical interviews with mothers prior to the child's matriculation in kindergarten. Early abuse increased the risk of teacher-rated externalizing outcomes in Grades 3 and 4 by fourfold, and this effect could not be accounted for by confounded ecological or child factors. Abuse was associated with later processing patterns (encoding errors, hostile attributional biases, accessing of aggressive responses, and positive evaluations of aggression), which, in turn, predicted later externalizing outcomes.


Subject(s)
Child Abuse/psychology , Child Behavior Disorders/etiology , Cognition Disorders/etiology , Age of Onset , Child , Child Behavior Disorders/diagnosis , Child Welfare , Cognition Disorders/diagnosis , Family/psychology , Female , Humans , Male , Problem Solving , Socioeconomic Factors
20.
Child Dev ; 65(2 Spec No): 649-65, 1994 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8013245

ABSTRACT

The goal was to examine processes in socialization that might account for an observed relation between early socioeconomic status and later child behavior problems. A representative sample of 585 children (n = 51 from the lowest socioeconomic class) was followed from preschool to grade 3. Socioeconomic status assessed in preschool significantly predicted teacher-rated externalizing problems and peer-rated aggressive behavior in kindergarten and grades 1, 2, and 3. Socioeconomic status was significantly negatively correlated with 8 factors in the child's socialization and social context, including harsh discipline, lack of maternal warmth, exposure to aggressive adult models, maternal aggressive values, family life stressors, mother's lack of social support, peer group instability, and lack of cognitive stimulation. These factors, in turn, significantly predicted teacher-rated externalizing problems and peer-nominated aggression and accounted for over half of the total effect of socioeconomic status on these outcomes. These findings suggest that part of the effect of socioeconomic status on children's aggressive development may be mediated by status-related socializing experiences.


Subject(s)
Child Behavior Disorders/psychology , Poverty/psychology , Socialization , Aggression/psychology , Child , Child Behavior Disorders/prevention & control , Child of Impaired Parents/psychology , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Internal-External Control , Life Change Events , Male , Minority Groups/psychology , Mother-Child Relations , Parenting/psychology , Personality Assessment , Risk Factors , Social Support , Sociometric Techniques
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...