Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 9 de 9
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Sci Rep ; 7(1): 15652, 2017 Nov 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29142226

ABSTRACT

People consistently show preferences and behaviors that benefit others at a cost to themselves, a phenomenon termed altruism. We investigated if perception of one's body signals - interoception - may be underlying such behaviors. We tested if participants' sensitivity to their own heartbeat predicted their decision on a choice between self-interest and altruism, and if improving this sensitivity through training would make participants more altruistic. Across these two experiments, interoceptive sensitivity predicted altruism measured through monetary generosity. Improving interoceptive sensitivity did, however, not lead to more altruistic behaviour. We conclude that there is a unique link between interoception and altruistic behaviour, likely established over an individual's history of altruistic acts, and the body responses they elicit. The findings suggest that humans might literally 'listen to their heart' to guide their altruistic behavior.


Subject(s)
Altruism , Interoception/physiology , Perception/physiology , Social Behavior , Decision Making , Female , Heart Rate/physiology , Humans , Male , Video Games/psychology
2.
Sci Rep ; 7: 43645, 2017 03 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28255172

ABSTRACT

Visual stimuli with emotional content appearing in close temporal proximity either before or after a target stimulus can hinder conscious perceptual processing of the target via an emotional attentional blink (EAB). This occurs for targets that appear after the emotional stimulus (forward EAB) and for those appearing before the emotional stimulus (retroactive EAB). Additionally, the traditional attentional blink (AB) occurs because detection of any target hinders detection of a subsequent target. The present study investigated the relations between these different attentional processes. Rapid sequences of landscape images were presented to thirty-one male participants with occasional landscape targets (rotated images). For the forward EAB, emotional or neutral distractor images of people were presented before the target; for the retroactive EAB, such images were also targets and presented after the landscape target. In the latter case, this design allowed investigation of the AB as well. Erotic and gory images caused more EABs than neutral images, but there were no differential effects on the AB. This pattern is striking because while using different target categories (rotated landscapes, people) appears to have eliminated the AB, the retroactive EAB still occurred, offering additional evidence for the power of emotional stimuli over conscious attention.


Subject(s)
Attention , Attentional Blink , Emotions , Adolescent , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation , Reaction Time , Young Adult
3.
Acta Psychol (Amst) ; 167: 1-15, 2016 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27065011

ABSTRACT

Task demands that influence scanning behaviour in one task can cause that behaviour to persist to a second unrelated task (carry over). This can also affect performance on a second task (e.g., hazard perception ratings), and has been attributed to a process of attentional bias that is modulated by top-down influences (Thompson & Crundall, 2011). In a series of experiments we explored how these top-down influences impact upon carry over. In all experiments, participants searched letters that were presented horizontally, vertically, or in a random array. They were then presented with a driving scene and rated the hazardousness of the scene. Carry over of eye-movements from the letter search to the scene was observed in all experiments. Furthermore, it was demonstrated that this carry over effect influenced hazard perception accuracy. The magnitude of carry over was correlated with task switching abilities, attentional conflicting, and attentional orienting (Experiment 1), and was affected by predictability of the primary task (Experiment 2). Furthermore, direct current stimulation of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and parietal areas affected the magnitude of the effect (Experiment 3). These results indicate that carry over is modulated by the specific ability to orient attention and disengage from this orientation. Over orienting leads to increased carry over and insufficient task switching is detrimental to task performance. As a result the current experiments provide evidence that the carry over effect is strongly influenced by attentional processes, namely orienting, inhibition, and task switching.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Eye Movements , Task Performance and Analysis , Adult , Female , Humans , Inhibition, Psychological , Male , Orientation/physiology , Random Allocation , Young Adult
4.
Neuropsychologia ; 49(12): 3314-9, 2011 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21884712

ABSTRACT

The importance of cues signaling reward, threat or danger would suggest that they receive processing privileges in the neural systems underlying perception and attention. Previous research has documented enhanced processing of motivationally salient cues, and has pointed to the amygdala as a candidate neural structure underlying the enhancements. In the current study, we examined whether the amygdala was necessary for this emotional modulation of attention to occur. Patients with unilateral amygdala lesions and matched controls completed an emotional attentional blink task in which emotional distractors impair the perception of subsequent targets. Emotional images proved more distracting across all participant groups, including those with right or left amygdala lesions. These data argue against a central role for the amygdala in mediating all types of attentional capture by emotional stimuli.


Subject(s)
Affective Symptoms/etiology , Amygdala/physiopathology , Attentional Blink/physiology , Brain Injuries/complications , Brain Injuries/pathology , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Cues , Female , Functional Laterality , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged , Photic Stimulation , Reaction Time
5.
Neuropsychologia ; 49(4): 596-601, 2011 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21414462

ABSTRACT

Previous research has indicated that the amygdala is a critical neural substrate of the emotional modulation of attention. However, a recent case study suggests that the amygdala may not be essential for all types of emotion-attention interactions. In order to test this hypothesis, we assessed the visual-search performance of patients with unilateral amygdala lesions, matched controls, and medication-matched epilepsy patients with intact amygdalae. All participants completed a visual-search task consisting of trials in which (1) an emotional target was embedded among neutral distractors, (2) a neutral target was embedded among emotional distractors, or (3) a neutral target was embedded among neutral distractors. All participant groups, including those with amygdala lesions, detected emotional targets more efficiently than neutral targets. These data indicate that the amygdala is not necessary for emotion-guided visual search and suggest that other mechanisms beyond the amygdala help guide attention toward threatening stimuli.

6.
Neuropsychologia ; 48(12): 3430-5, 2010 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20637217

ABSTRACT

Previous research has indicated that the amygdala is a critical neural substrate of the emotional modulation of attention. However, a recent case study suggests that the amygdala may not be essential for all types of emotion-attention interactions. In order to test this hypothesis, we assessed the visual-search performance of patients with unilateral amygdala lesions, matched controls, and medication-matched epilepsy patients with intact amygdalae. All participants completed a visual-search task consisting of trials in which (1) an emotional target was embedded among neutral distractors, (2) a neutral target was embedded among emotional distractors, or (3) a neutral target was embedded among neutral distractors. All participant groups, including those with amygdala lesions, detected emotional targets more efficiently than neutral targets. These data indicate that the amygdala is not necessary for emotion-guided visual search and suggest that other mechanisms beyond the amygdala help guide attention toward threatening stimuli.


Subject(s)
Amygdala/pathology , Attention/physiology , Brain Injuries/physiopathology , Fear/psychology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Brain Injuries/pathology , Female , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Male , Middle Aged , Photic Stimulation/methods , Reaction Time/physiology , Young Adult
7.
Appetite ; 54(3): 579-82, 2010 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19914320

ABSTRACT

While effects of hunger on motivation and food reward value are well-established, far less is known about the effects of hunger on cognitive processes. Here, we deployed the emotional blink of attention paradigm to investigate the impact of visual food cues on attentional capture under conditions of hunger and satiety. Participants were asked to detect targets which appeared in a rapid visual stream after different types of task irrelevant distractors. We observed that food stimuli acquired increased power to capture attention and prevent target detection when participants were hungry. This occurred despite monetary incentives to perform well. Our findings suggest an attentional mechanism through which hunger heightens perception of food cues. As an objective behavioral marker of the attentional sensitivity to food cues, the emotional attentional blink paradigm may provide a useful technique for studying individual differences, and state manipulations in the sensitivity to food cues.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Food , Hunger/physiology , Vision, Ocular , Arousal/physiology , Eating/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Satiation/physiology
8.
Brain Cogn ; 72(2): 282-8, 2010 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19857920

ABSTRACT

Making the right choice depends crucially on the accurate valuation of the available options in the light of current needs and goals of an individual. Thus, the valuation of identical options can vary considerably with motivational context. The present study investigated the neural structures underlying context dependent evaluation. We instructed participants to choose from food menu items based on different criteria: on their anticipated taste or on ease of preparation. The aim of the manipulation was to assess which neural sites were activated during choice guided by incentive value, and which during choice based on a value-irrelevant criterion. To assess the impact of increased motivation, affect-guided choice and cognition-guided choice was compared during the sated and hungry states. During affective choice, we identified increased activity in structures representing primarily valuation and taste (medial prefrontal cortex, insula). During cognitive choice, structures showing increased activity included those implicated in suppression and conflict monitoring (lateral orbitofrontal cortex, anterior cingulate). Hunger influenced choice-related activity in the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. Our results show that choice is associated with the use of distinct neural structures for the pursuit of different goals.


Subject(s)
Affect/physiology , Brain/physiology , Choice Behavior/physiology , Cognition/physiology , Feeding Behavior/physiology , Motivation , Adult , Brain Mapping , Female , Humans , Hunger/physiology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Satiation/physiology
9.
PLoS One ; 4(8): e6581, 2009 Aug 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19672296

ABSTRACT

How much we desire a meal depends on both the constituent foods and how hungry we are, though not every meal becomes more desirable with increasing hunger. The brain therefore needs to be able to integrate hunger and meal properties to compute the correct incentive value of a meal. The present study investigated the functional role of the amygdala and the orbitofrontal cortex in mediating hunger and dish attractiveness. Furthermore, it explored neural responses to dish descriptions particularly susceptible to value-increase following fasting. We instructed participants to rate how much they wanted food menu items while they were either hungry or sated, and compared the rating differences in these states. Our results point to the representation of food value in the amygdala, and to an integration of attractiveness with hunger level in the orbitofrontal cortex. Dishes particularly desirable during hunger activated the thalamus and the insula. Our results specify the functions of evaluative structures in the context of food attractiveness, and point to a complex neural representation of dish qualities which contribute to state-dependent value.


Subject(s)
Appetite , Brain/physiology , Hunger , Adult , Eating , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...