Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 6 de 6
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Behav Brain Sci ; 47: e21, 2024 Jan 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38224082

ABSTRACT

Although Glowacki proposed that peace developed from the relatively recent advent of intergroup norms and tolerance for out-group members, we submit that (a) positive intergroup relations developed from a psychology grounded in the regulation of intragroup relations, (b) the "default" intergroup orientation is uncertainty, and (c) positive intergroup relations likely existed early in our evolutionary history.


Subject(s)
Group Processes , Interpersonal Relations , Humans
2.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull ; 40(4): 513-25, 2014 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24398541

ABSTRACT

The multifaceted-self effect is the ascription of more traits to self than others. Consensus is that this effect occurs for positive, but not negative, traits. We propose that the effect also occurs for negative traits when they can be endorsed with low intensity ("I am a little bit lazy"), thereby circumventing self-protection concerns. In Experiment 1, the multifaceted-self effect occurred for positive, but not negative, traits on a high-intensity trait-endorsement measure. However, it occurred irrespective of trait valence on a low-intensity trait-endorsement measure. In Experiment 2, the multifaceted-self effect occurred for positive, but not negative, traits on a strong trait-endorsement measure. However, it occurred irrespective of trait valence on a diminuted trait-endorsement measure--a finding conceptually replicated in Experiment 3. In Experiment 4, participants spontaneously adopted diminutive terms ("a little bit") when describing their negative traits. Individuals reconcile negative self-knowledge with self-protection concerns by expressing it in muted terms.


Subject(s)
Self Concept , Adolescent , Adult , Affect , Aged , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Personality , Young Adult
3.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 93(2): 250-65, 2007 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17645398

ABSTRACT

Two experiments contrasted interactions between group leaders with interactions between individuals in a mixed-motive setting. Consistent with the idea that being accountable to the in-group implies normative pressure to benefit the in-group, Experiment 1 found that accountable leaders were more competitive than individuals. Consistent with the idea that being unaccountable to the in-group implies normative pressure to be cooperative and that high guilt proneness provides motivation to be moral, Experiment 2 found that when guilt proneness was high, unaccountable leaders were less competitive than accountable leaders and did not differ significantly from individuals. In other words, the robust interindividual-intergroup discontinuity effect was eliminated when groups had unaccountable leaders who were high in guilt proneness.


Subject(s)
Cooperative Behavior , Guilt , Interpersonal Relations , Leadership , Social Responsibility , Adult , Female , Humans , Male
4.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 88(2): 365-85, 2005 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15841864

ABSTRACT

Two experiments used a 3-choice variation of the prisoner's dilemma game to explore the paradoxical implications of expected cooperation of other groups and individuals for competitiveness and cooperativeness. Experiment 1 found that an experimental manipulation of opponent trustworthiness influenced the tendency of both groups and individuals to cooperate but had no significant effect on the tendency of groups and individuals to compete--possibly because of the perceived unfairness of competing with a trustworthy other. Experiment 2 found that an experimental manipulation of categorization increased the tendency of same-category groups and individuals to cooperate (as in Experiment 1) and also increased the tendency of same-category groups, but not individuals, to compete (unlike in Experiment 1). It was further found that the tendency of same-category groups to compete more than same-category individuals was relatively stronger for participants high in guilt proneness.


Subject(s)
Attitude , Cooperative Behavior , Interpersonal Relations , Social Behavior , Social Perception , Choice Behavior , Competitive Behavior , Female , Guilt , Humans , Male , Surveys and Questionnaires
5.
Exp Psychol ; 52(1): 74-9, 2005.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15779533

ABSTRACT

A. Karpinski (2004) recently criticized Implicit Association Test (IAT) measures of self-esteem, arguing that their measurements of self-associations are compromised by their contrasting self with a putatively extremely negative second category, the nonspecific other. The present data show, to the contrary, that the nonspecific other category in the self-esteem IAT is near neutral in valence. Validity of the self-esteem IAT is most appropriately assessed by examining its correlations with conceptually related measures. That has been done in several previous studies that are reviewed here. The nonspecific other category is only one of several choices for representing the concept of other in self-esteem IATs. Choice of the appropriate other category to contrast with self in self-esteem IATs should be guided by the needs of the research question being addressed.


Subject(s)
Psychological Tests , Self Concept , Association , Female , Humans , Male , Reproducibility of Results
6.
Psychol Bull ; 129(5): 698-722, 2003 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12956540

ABSTRACT

This quantitative review of 130 comparisons of interindividual and intergroup interactions in the context of mixed-motive situations reveals that intergroup interactions are generally more competitive than interindividual interactions. The authors identify 4 moderators of this interindividual-intergroup discontinuity effect, each based on the theoretical perspective that the discontinuity effect flows from greater fear and greed in intergroup relative to interindividual interactions. Results reveal that each moderator shares a unique association with the magnitude of the discontinuity effect. The discontinuity effect is larger when (a) participants interact with an opponent whose behavior is unconstrained by the experimenter or constrained by the experimenter to be cooperative rather than constrained by the experimenter to be reciprocal, (b) group members make a group decision rather than individual decisions, (c) unconstrained communication between participants is present rather than absent, and (d) conflict of interest is severe rather than mild.


Subject(s)
Group Processes , Social Behavior , Social Perception , Fear , Humans , Models, Psychological
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...