Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 6 de 6
Filter
1.
J Child Sex Abus ; 20(6): 677-94, 2011 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22126110

ABSTRACT

Eighty-five New Zealand based practitioners experienced in treating adults with a history of child sexual abuse participated in an online judgment study of child sexual abuse outcomes using signal detection theory methodology. Participants' level of sensitivity was assessed independent of their degree of response bias when discriminating (a) known child sexual abuse outcomes from behaviors thought to be unrelated to child sexual abuse and (b) direct child sexual abuse effects from subsequent coping strategies. Results demonstrated good sensitivity (accuracy) when identifying child sexual abuse effects from noneffects. When asked to discriminate direct child sexual abuse effects from ways of coping with distress, practitioners' accuracy was reduced, revealing a tendency to identify all effects as coping. Although treatment approaches highlight the pivotal role of identifying coping strategies, practitioners did not perceive maladaptive coping as a distinct clinical feature. Complex abuse cases may benefit from replacing maladaptive coping strategies (e.g., self-harm) with constructive coping (e.g., social support) in order to deliver efficacious practice.


Subject(s)
Adult Survivors of Child Abuse/psychology , Attitude of Health Personnel , Child Abuse, Sexual/psychology , Crime Victims/psychology , Physical Examination/psychology , Physician-Patient Relations , Adaptation, Psychological , Adult , Adult Survivors of Child Abuse/statistics & numerical data , Child , Child Abuse, Sexual/diagnosis , Child Abuse, Sexual/statistics & numerical data , Defense Mechanisms , Female , Humans , Life Change Events , Male , Middle Aged , New Zealand , Surveys and Questionnaires
2.
Behav Res Ther ; 45(4): 805-18, 2007 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16962560

ABSTRACT

The present study explores driving skills in a group of 50 media-recruited driving-fearful and 50 control drivers, all of whom were women. Participants completed an on-road practical driving assessment with a professional driving instructor. Diagnostic as well as pre-post self-report and instructor driving assessments were conducted. Fearful drivers made more errors on the driving assessment than controls. However, the pattern of errors was identical for both groups, indicating that fear and anxiety may be associated with the number rather than the type of driving errors made. These differences remained when factors such as driving history, current driving frequency, and diagnosis were controlled using case selection. More research is needed to replicate the findings in more diverse samples. Additional work should also aim to clarify the specific role of driving skills in driving fear, which will facilitate treatment planning for exposure-based treatments and help identify cases where driving skills assessment may be appropriate.


Subject(s)
Automobile Driving/psychology , Fear , Accidents, Traffic/psychology , Adult , Anxiety , Automobile Driver Examination , Automobile Driving/standards , Female , Humans , Middle Aged , Psychometrics , Psychomotor Performance
3.
J Anxiety Disord ; 21(4): 493-509, 2007.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16982173

ABSTRACT

Recent research has suggested that fear of driving is common in the general population. People may have various concerns when driving, and instruments for the assessment of these concerns are lacking. The present paper describes the development and preliminary evaluation of the Driving Cognitions Questionnaire (DCQ). The DCQ is a 20-item scale that measures three areas of driving-related concerns--panic-related, accident-related, and social concerns. In three separate samples from different countries (n=69, 100, and 78), the scale showed good internal consistency and substantial correlations with measures of the severity of driving fear. It discriminated well between people with and without driving phobia. It also showed convergent validity with other measures. The questionnaire shows promise for use in research and clinical practice.


Subject(s)
Automobile Driving/psychology , Cognition , Phobic Disorders/diagnosis , Phobic Disorders/psychology , Psychological Tests , Accidents, Traffic/psychology , Agoraphobia/psychology , Case-Control Studies , Cognitive Behavioral Therapy , Factor Analysis, Statistical , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Panic Disorder/psychology , Psychometrics , Reproducibility of Results , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/psychology
4.
Rehabil Nurs ; 30(5): 207-12, 2005.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16175926

ABSTRACT

Guillain-Barré syndrome is a rare neurological disease that causes paralysis and may necessitate hospitalization for some patients in its acute stages. It primarily affects the peripheral nervous system, though recent research has shown that for some patients, the central nervous system is involved. The acute phase often requires intensive care services. Recognition is growing that recovery is not as smooth and free of symptoms as previously thought. Following "recovery" some people endure long-term residual symptoms, such as fatigue and pain. Nursing input can be of value by providing support, information, explanations, and empathy to reassure patients and families. A greater understanding of the nature and course of the disease and its ramifications can lead to more effective nursing management and a faster rehabilitation process.


Subject(s)
Central Nervous System Diseases , Guillain-Barre Syndrome , Central Nervous System Diseases/nursing , Central Nervous System Diseases/physiopathology , Central Nervous System Diseases/rehabilitation , Guillain-Barre Syndrome/nursing , Guillain-Barre Syndrome/physiopathology , Guillain-Barre Syndrome/rehabilitation , Humans
5.
Clin Neuropsychol ; 18(3): 433-48, 2004 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15739814

ABSTRACT

Little empirical research has examined guidelines for neuropsychological assessment of people with severe physical and sensory disabilities. This study focused on people with expressive disabilities--people unable to speak, write, draw, or manipulate test materials. Measures were selected and adapted to be suitable for the assessment of cognitive functioning for such people. Responses were selected from multiple choice answers or were spelled out on an alphabet board. Individuals with expressive disabilities in addition to traumatic brain injuiry (TBI) were compared to individuals with TBI alone and a normative group. The measures were generally found to be reliable and valid assessment instruments. Individuals with expressive disabilities were able to manage the task requirements of the adapted tests. The results provided support for the adaptations trialed.


Subject(s)
Brain Injuries/complications , Brain Injuries/diagnosis , Communication Disorders/diagnosis , Communication Disorders/etiology , Disability Evaluation , Neuropsychological Tests , Adolescent , Adult , Confidence Intervals , Female , Humans , Male , Mental Processes/physiology , Middle Aged , Psychometrics/methods , Reproducibility of Results , Retrospective Studies
6.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 10(4): 843-76, 2003 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15000533

ABSTRACT

It has been known for over 40 years that there are two fundamentally different kinds of detection tasks in the theory of signal detectability. The Type 1 task is to distinguish between events defined independently of the observer; the Type 2 task is to distinguish between one's own correct and incorrect decisions about those Type 1 events. For the Type 1 task, the behavior of the detector can be summarized by the traditional receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve. This curve can be compared with a theoretical ROC curve, which can be generated from overlapping probability functions conditional on the Type 1 events on an appropriate decision axis. We show how to derive the probability functions underlying Type 2 decisions from those for the Type 1 task. ROC curves and the usual measures of performance are readily obtained from those Type 2 functions, and some relationships among various Type 1 and Type 2 performance measures are presented. We discuss the relationship between Type 1 and Type 2 confidence ratings and caution against the practice of presenting transformed Type 2 ratings as empirical Type 1 ratings.


Subject(s)
Decision Making , Discrimination Learning , Observer Variation , Signal Detection, Psychological , Cues , Humans , Likelihood Functions , Models, Statistical , Probability Learning , Problem Solving , ROC Curve
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...