Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 10 de 10
Filter
1.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 74(6): 1054-1069, 2021 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33427086

ABSTRACT

People are likely to use their own knowledge as a frame of reference when they try to assess another person's perspective. Due to this egocentric anchoring, people often overestimate the extent to which others share their point of view. This study investigated which type of feedback (if any) stimulates perceivers to make estimations of another person's perspective that are less biased by egocentric knowledge. We allocated participants to one of the three feedback conditions (no feedback, accuracy feedback, narrative feedback). Findings showed that participants who were given feedback adjusted their perspective-judgement more than those who did not receive feedback. They also showed less egocentric projection on future assessments. Participants adjusted their perspective within the same trial to the same degree for both feedback types. However, participants' egocentric bias was only reduced when they received narrative feedback and not when they received accuracy feedback about their performance. Implications of these findings for theories of perspective-taking are discussed.


Subject(s)
Judgment , Adolescent , Adult , Bias , Feedback , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
2.
J Lang Soc Psychol ; 37(4): 497-517, 2018 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30443097

ABSTRACT

According to common belief, friends communicate more accurately and efficiently than strangers, because they can use uniquely shared knowledge and common knowledge to explain things to each other, while strangers are restricted to common knowledge. To test this belief, we asked friends and strangers to play, via e-mail and face-to-face, the word-description game Taboo, in which objects need to be described without using certain "taboo" words. When descriptions were sent via e-mail, there was no difference in accuracy (number of correct answers) nor in efficiency (number of words per correct answer) between friends and strangers. When descriptions were given face-to-face, friends were more accurate than strangers, but not more efficient (number of seconds and words per correct answer). Shared knowledge did not predict accuracy or efficiency. Hence, our findings do not support the idea that friends only need a few words to understand each other.

3.
PLoS One ; 8(11): e79039, 2013.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24236087

ABSTRACT

Although the name-letter-effect has been demonstrated reliably in choice contexts, recent research has called into question the existence of the name-letter-effect-the tendency among people to make choices that bear remarkable similarity with the letters in their own name. In this paper, we propose a connection between the name-letter-effect and interpersonal, group-level behavior that has not been previously captured in the literature. Specifically, we suggest that sharing initials with other group members promotes positive feelings toward those group members that in turn affect group outcomes. Using both field and laboratory studies, we found that sharing initials with group members cause groups to perform better by demonstrating greater performance, collective efficacy, adaptive conflict, and accuracy (on a hidden-profile task). Although many studies have investigated the effects of member similarity on various outcomes, our research demonstrates how minimal a degree of similarity among members is sufficient to influence quality of group outcomes.


Subject(s)
Cooperative Behavior , Names , Adaptation, Physiological , Group Processes , Humans , Quality Improvement , Task Performance and Analysis
4.
PLoS One ; 8(12): e81643, 2013.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24386082

ABSTRACT

There is a widespread belief that women are better at selecting gifts than men; however, this claim has not been assessed on the basis of objective criteria. The current studies do exactly that and show that women do indeed make better gift selections for others, regardless of the gender of the receiver and the type of relationship between the giver and receiver. We investigate the mediating role of different aspects of interpersonal sensitivity and reveal that differences in interpersonal interest (measured with an autism questionnaire), but not differences in interpersonal reactivity, explain gender differences in gift selection quality. The current studies thus present the first objective evidence for the claim that women are better in selecting gifts for others and also give an indication of why this is the case.


Subject(s)
Gift Giving , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Female , Humans , Male , Sex Factors
5.
J Autism Dev Disord ; 42(10): 2252-6, 2012 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22350338

ABSTRACT

Individuals with autism spectrum disorders or autistic traits may profit from Internet and computer-mediated interactions, but there is concern about their Internet use becoming compulsive. This study investigated the link between autistic traits and Internet use in a 2-wave longitudinal study with a non-clinical community sample (n = 390). As compared to people with less autistic traits, people with more autistic traits did not report a higher frequency of Internet use, but they were more prone to compulsive Internet use. For women, more autistic traits predicted an increase in compulsive Internet use over time. These results suggest that, despite its appeal for people with autistic traits, the Internet carries the risk of compulsive use.


Subject(s)
Autistic Disorder/psychology , Compulsive Behavior/psychology , Internet , Adult , Female , Humans , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Sex Factors
6.
Psychol Sci ; 22(9): 1191-7, 2011 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21771963

ABSTRACT

Data from a large survey of 1,561 professionals were used to examine the relationship between power and infidelity and the process underlying this relationship. Results showed that elevated power is positively associated with infidelity because power increases confidence in the ability to attract partners. This association was found for both actual infidelity and intentions to engage in infidelity in the future. Gender did not moderate these results: The relationship between power and infidelity was the same for women as for men, and for the same reason. These findings suggest that the common assumption (and often-found effect) that women are less likely than men to engage in infidelity is, at least partially, a reflection of traditional gender-based differences in power that exist in society.


Subject(s)
Extramarital Relations/psychology , Power, Psychological , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Sex Factors , Surveys and Questionnaires
7.
Exp Psychol ; 57(6): 412-8, 2010.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20178935

ABSTRACT

Mimicry and prosocial feelings are generally thought to be positively related. However, the conditions under which mimicry and liking are related largely remain unspecified. We advance this specification by examining the relationship between mimicry and liking more thoroughly. In two experiments, we manipulated an individual's a priori liking for another and investigated whether it influenced mimicry of that person. Our experiments demonstrate that in the presence of a reason to like a target, automatic mimicry is increased. However, mimicry did not decrease when disliking a target. These studies provide further evidence of a link between mimicry and liking and extend previous research by showing that a certain level of mimicry even occurs when mimicry behavior is inconsistent with one's goals or motivations.


Subject(s)
Emotions/physiology , Imitative Behavior , Interpersonal Relations , Adolescent , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Facial Expression , Female , Humans , Male , Motivation , Regression Analysis , Social Behavior , Surveys and Questionnaires
8.
J Autism Dev Disord ; 40(4): 470-8, 2010 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19885724

ABSTRACT

People with ASD have deficits in their social skills and may therefore experience lower relationship satisfaction. This study investigated possible mechanisms to explain whether and how autistic traits, measured with the AQ, influence relationship satisfaction in a non-clinical sample of 195 married couples. More autistic traits were associated with lower relationship satisfaction for husbands but not for wives. Multiple mediation analyses revealed that husbands' responsiveness towards their wives, trust, and intimacy mediated this link between autistic traits and relationship satisfaction. These findings suggest that autistic traits may hamper men's relationship satisfaction because they impede relationship-specific feelings and behavior. There was no partner-effect of autistic traits, indicating that more autistic traits do not necessarily influence the partner's perceptions of relationship satisfaction.


Subject(s)
Autistic Disorder/psychology , Interpersonal Relations , Marriage/psychology , Personal Satisfaction , Spouses/psychology , Adult , Female , Humans , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Object Attachment , Self Concept , Severity of Illness Index , Social Behavior , Surveys and Questionnaires
9.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull ; 35(11): 1512-27, 2009 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19638635

ABSTRACT

Understanding is at the heart of intimate relationships. It is unclear, however, whether understanding-partners' subjective feeling that they understand each other-or knowledge-partners' accurate knowledge of each other-is more important for relationship well-being. The present article pits these two types of understanding against each other and investigates their effects on relationship well-being. In a prospective study among 199 newlywed couples, partners' self-reported and perceived understanding and their knowledge in different domains were assessed. Understanding was independent of knowledge. Self-reported and perceived understanding predicted relationship well-being but neither type of knowledge did. Thus, subjectively feeling that one understands and is understood by one's partner appears to be more important to relationship well-being than actually knowing and being known by one's partner.


Subject(s)
Comprehension , Emotions , Empathy , Interpersonal Relations , Love , Marriage/psychology , Adaptation, Psychological , Adult , Communication , Female , Humans , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Object Attachment
10.
Pers Soc Psychol Bull ; 33(8): 1152-66, 2007 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17565049

ABSTRACT

Using extensive diary data from people taking their driver's license exam, the authors investigated the role of time in affective forecasting accuracy. Replicating existing findings, participants grossly overestimated the intensity and duration of their negative affect after failure and only slightly overestimated the intensity and duration of their positive affect after success. Extending existing findings, participants accurately predicted a decrease of their affective reactions over time but underestimated the speed with which this decrease would occur. In addition, they showed greater forecasting accuracy for positive affect than negative affect when the exam was distant and greater forecasting accuracy for negative affect than positive affect when the exam was close. The motivational processes underlying these findings are being discussed.


Subject(s)
Achievement , Automobile Driver Examination/psychology , Forecasting , Task Performance and Analysis , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Netherlands , Surveys and Questionnaires
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...