Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 4 de 4
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 26(3): 459-75, 2014 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24116838

ABSTRACT

How does reading expertise change the visual system? Here, we explored whether the visual system could develop dedicated perceptual mechanisms in early and intermediate visual cortex under the pressure for fast processing that is particularly strong in reading. We compared fMRI activations in Chinese participants with limited knowledge of French and in French participants with no knowledge of Chinese, exploiting these doubly dissociated reading skills as a tool to study the neural correlates of visual expertise. All participants viewed the same stimuli: words in both languages and matched visual controls, presented at a fast rate comparable with fluent reading. In the Visual Word Form Area, all participants showed enhanced responses to their known scripts. However, group differences were found in occipital cortex. In French readers reading French, activations were enhanced in left-hemisphere visual area V1, with the strongest differences between French words and their controls found at the central and horizontal meridian representations. Chinese participants, who were not expert French readers, did not show these early visual activations. In contrast, Chinese readers reading Chinese showed enhanced activations in intermediate visual areas V3v/hV4, absent in French participants. Together with our previous findings [Szwed, M., Dehaene, S., Kleinschmidt, A., Eger, E., Valabregue, R., Amadon, A., et al. Specialization for written words over objects in the visual cortex. Neuroimage, 56, 330-344, 2011], our results suggest that the effects of extensive practice can be found at the lowest levels of the visual system. They also reveal their cross-script variability: Alphabetic reading involves enhanced engagement of central and right meridian V1 representations that are particularly used in left-to-right reading, whereas Chinese characters put greater emphasis on intermediate visual areas.


Subject(s)
Language , Occipital Lobe/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Reading , Temporal Lobe/physiology , Visual Cortex/physiology , Adult , Attention/physiology , Brain/physiology , Brain Mapping , China , Female , France , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Photic Stimulation , Practice, Psychological , Visual Pathways/physiology , Vocabulary , Young Adult
2.
J Vis ; 11(6): 1-21, 2011 May 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21566152

ABSTRACT

Identifying letters and their relative positions is the basis of reading in literate adults. The Local Combinations Detector model hypothesizes that this ability results from the general organization of the visual system, whereby object encoding proceeds through a hierarchy of neural detectors that, in the case of reading, would be tuned to letters, bigrams, or other letter combinations. Given the increase of receptive fields by a factor of 2 to 3 from one neural level to the next, detectors should integrate information only for letters separated by at most 2 other characters. We test this prediction by measuring the impact of letter spacing on reading, purifying this effect from confounding variables. We establish that performance deteriorates non-linearly whenever letters are separated by at least 2 blank spaces, with the concomitant emergence of a word length effect. We then show that this cannot be reduced to an effect of physical size nor of visual eccentricity. Finally, we demonstrate that the threshold of about 2 spaces is constant across variations in font size. Those results support the hypothesis that the fast recognition of combinations of nearby letters plays a central role in the coding of words, such that interfering with this representation prevents the parallel analysis of letter strings.


Subject(s)
Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Perceptual Masking/physiology , Reading , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
3.
Neuroimage ; 49(2): 1786-99, 2010 Jan 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19781648

ABSTRACT

Expert readers exhibit a remarkable ability to recognize handwriting, in spite of enormous variability in character shape-a competence whose cerebral underpinnings are unknown. Subliminal priming, combined with neuroimaging, can reveal which brain areas automatically compute an invariant representation of visual stimuli. Here, we used behavioral and fMRI priming to study the areas involved in invariant handwritten word recognition. Compared to printed words, easily readable handwritten words caused additional activity in ventral occipitotemporal cortex, particularly in the right hemisphere, while difficult handwriting also mobilized an attentional parietofrontal network. Remarkably, however, subliminal repetition effects were observed across printed and handwritten styles, whether easy or difficult to read, both behaviorally and in the activation of the left visual word form area (VWFA). These results indicate that the left inferotemporal VWFA possesses an unsuspected degree of fast and automatic visual invariance for handwritten words, although surprisingly this invariance can be reflected both as repetition suppression and as repetition enhancement.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Handwriting , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Reading , Visual Pathways/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Female , Habituation, Psychophysiologic/physiology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Photic Stimulation , Reaction Time , Task Performance and Analysis , Young Adult
4.
Vision Res ; 49(7): 718-25, 2009 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19200808

ABSTRACT

Object recognition relies heavily on invariant visual features such as the manner in which lines meet at vertices to form viewpoint-invariant junctions (e.g. T, L). We wondered whether these features also underlie readers' competence for fast recognition of printed words. Since reading is far too recent to have exerted any evolutionary pressure on brain evolution, visual word recognition might be based on pre-existing mechanisms common to all visual object recognition. In a naming task, we presented partially deleted pictures of objects and printed words in which either the vertices or the line midsegments were preserved. Subjects showed an identical pattern of behavior with both objects and words: they made fewer errors and were faster to respond when vertices were preserved. Our results suggest that vertex invariants are used for object recognition and that this evolutionarily ancient mechanism is being co-opted for reading.


Subject(s)
Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Reading , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Orientation , Photic Stimulation/methods , Semantics , Young Adult
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...