Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 4 de 4
Filter
Add more filters











Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Br J Psychiatry ; 179: 144-50, 2001 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11483476

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Better indicators are needed for identifying children with early signs of developmental psychopathology. AIMS: To identify measures of autonomic nervous system reactivity that discriminate children with internalising and externalising behavioural symptoms. METHOD: A cross-sectional study of 122 children aged 6--7 years examined sympathetic and parasympathetic reactivity to standardised field-laboratory stressors as predictors of parent- and teacher-reported mental health symptoms. RESULTS: Measures of autonomic reactivity discriminated between children with internalising behaviour problems, externalising behaviour problems and neither. Internalisers showed high reactivity relative to low-symptom children, principally in the parasympathetic branch, while externalisers showed low reactivity, in both autonomic branches. CONCLUSIONS: School-age children with mental health symptoms showed a pattern of autonomic dimorphism in their reactivity to standardised challenges. This observation may be of use in early identification of children with presyndromal psychopathology.


Subject(s)
Arousal/physiology , Autonomic Nervous System Diseases/physiopathology , Child Behavior Disorders/physiopathology , Blood Pressure/physiology , Child , Child Behavior Disorders/diagnosis , Child Behavior Disorders/psychology , Cross-Sectional Studies , Female , Heart Rate/physiology , Humans , Male , Sex Factors , Statistics, Nonparametric , Stress, Psychological/physiopathology
2.
Dev Psychobiol ; 37(3): 153-75, 2000 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11044863

ABSTRACT

Although there are general assumptions that physiological and behavioral indices of emotion are interrelated, empirical research has revealed inconsistent findings with regard to their degree of association, particularly in children. Two studies were conducted to examine the relations between cardiovascular reactivity and emotional behavior. In the first study, 3- to 6-year-olds completed challenging tasks during which measures of their physiological responses and facial expressions were obtained. With age, children's heart rate decreased, vagal tone increased, and facial expressions became slightly more exaggerated. However, children's physiologic reactions were unrelated to their concurrent facial expression when all children were considered, when only boys were considered, and when children extreme in their physiologic reactions were considered. Only among girls was physiologic reactivity moderately associated with concurrent negative expressiveness. In the second study, 4- and 5-year-olds' physiologic reactivity was examined as a predictor of later overt emotional reaction to venipuncture episodes. Children's overt emotional reactions were consistent across repeated venipunctures, and girls were more visibly distressed than boys. As in the first study, physiologic reactivity was generally unrelated to children's behavioral responses. Findings have implications for assumptions about the degree of coupling between biological and behavioral emotional systems in childhood.


Subject(s)
Blood Pressure/physiology , Child Development/physiology , Expressed Emotion , Heart Rate/physiology , Phlebotomy/psychology , Stress, Psychological , Vagus Nerve/physiology , Age Factors , Analysis of Variance , Child , Child, Preschool , Cohort Studies , Electrocardiography , Facial Expression , Female , Humans , Male , Models, Biological , Sex Factors
3.
J Exp Child Psychol ; 72(4): 235-70, 1999 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10074380

ABSTRACT

Children's memories for an experienced and a never-experienced medical procedure were examined. Three- to 13-year-olds were questioned about a voiding cystourethrogram fluoroscopy (VCUG) they endured between 2 and 6 years of age. Children 4 years or older at VCUG were more accurate than children younger than 4 at VCUG. Longer delays were associated with providing fewer units of correct information but not with more inaccuracies. Parental avoidant attachment style was related to increased errors in children's VCUG memory. Children were more likely to assent to the false medical procedure when it was alluded to briefly than when described in detail, and false assents were related to fewer "do-not-know" responses about the VCUG. Results have implications for childhood amnesia, stress and memory, individual differences, and eyewitness testimony.


Subject(s)
Affect/physiology , Memory Disorders/etiology , Retention, Psychology/physiology , Stress, Psychological/psychology , Suggestion , Adolescent , Age Factors , Child , Child, Preschool , Humans , Surveys and Questionnaires
4.
Child Abuse Negl ; 21(11): 1111-30, 1997 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9422831

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: The goals of the present study were to examine the extent of children's religious, especially satanic, knowledge and to understand the influence of children's age, religious training, family, and media exposure on that knowledge. METHODS: Using a structured interview, 48 3- to 16-year-old children were questioned about their knowledge of: (a) religion and religious worship; (b) religion-related symbols and pictures; and (c) movies, music, and television shows with religious and horror themes. RESULTS: Although few children evinced direct knowledge of ritual abuse, many revealed general knowledge of satanism and satanic worship. With age, children's religious knowledge increased and became more sophisticated. Increased exposure to nonsatanic horror media was associated with more nonreligious knowledge that could be considered precursory to satanic knowledge, and increased exposure to satanic media was associated with more knowledge related to satanism. CONCLUSIONS: Our results suggest that children do not generally possess sufficient knowledge of satanic ritual abuse to make up false allegations on their own. However, many children have knowledge of satanism as well as nonreligious knowledge of violence, death, and illegal activities. It is possible that such knowledge could prompt an investigation of satanic ritual abuse or possibly serve as a starting point from which an allegation is erected.


Subject(s)
Ceremonial Behavior , Child Abuse , Religion , Adolescent , Age Factors , Child , Child, Preschool , Family , Female , Humans , Male , Mass Media , Religious Philosophies , Surveys and Questionnaires , Symbolism
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL