Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 9 de 9
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 146(5): 3733, 2019 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31795701

ABSTRACT

It is well-established that excessive noise exposure can systematically shift audiometric thresholds (i.e., noise-induced hearing loss, NIHL) making sounds at the lower end of the dynamic range difficult to detect. An often overlooked symptom of NIHL is the degraded ability to resolve temporal fluctuations in supra-threshold signals. Given that the temporal properties of speech are highly dynamic, it is not surprising that NIHL greatly reduces one's ability to clearly decipher spoken language. However, systematic characterization of noise-induced impairments on supra-threshold signals in humans is difficult given the variability in noise exposure among individuals. Fortunately, the chinchilla is audiometrically similar to humans, making it an ideal animal model to investigate noise-induced supra-threshold deficits. Through a series of studies using the chinchilla, the authors have elucidated several noise-induced deficits in temporal processing that occur at supra-threshold levels. These experiments highlight the importance of the chinchilla model in developing an understanding of noise-induced deficits in temporal processing.


Subject(s)
Auditory Threshold , Hearing Loss, Noise-Induced/physiopathology , Reaction Time , Adaptation, Physiological , Animals , Avoidance Learning , Chinchilla , Cochlear Nerve/physiology , Cochlear Nerve/physiopathology
2.
Hear Res ; 332: 217-222, 2016 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26427583

ABSTRACT

The cannabinoid receptor 1 (CB1R) is found at several stages in the auditory pathway, but its role in hearing is unknown. Hearing abilities were measured in CB1R knockout mice and compared to those of wild-type mice. Operant conditioning and the psychophysical Method of Constant Stimuli were used to measure audiograms, gap detection thresholds, and frequency difference limens in trained mice using the same methods and stimuli as in previous experiments. CB1R knockout mice showed deficits at frequencies above 8 kHz in their audiograms relative to wild-type mice. CB1R knockouts showed enhancements for detecting gaps in low-pass noisebursts relative to wild-type mice, but were similar for other noise conditions. Finally, the two groups of mice did not differ in their frequency discrimination abilities as measured by the frequency difference limens task. These experiments suggest that the CB1R is involved in auditory processing and lay the groundwork for future physiological experiments.


Subject(s)
Auditory Pathways/metabolism , Auditory Perception , Behavior, Animal , Cues , Pitch Perception , Receptor, Cannabinoid, CB1/deficiency , Signal Detection, Psychological , Acoustic Stimulation , Animals , Audiometry, Pure-Tone , Auditory Pathways/physiopathology , Conditioning, Operant , Genotype , Mice, Inbred CBA , Mice, Knockout , Noise/adverse effects , Perceptual Masking , Phenotype , Receptor, Cannabinoid, CB1/genetics , Time Factors
3.
Front Neurol ; 6: 31, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25750635

ABSTRACT

To test the "tinnitus gap-filling" hypothesis in an animal psychoacoustic paradigm, rats were tested using a go/no-go operant gap detection task in which silent intervals of various durations were embedded within a continuous noise. Gap detection thresholds were measured before and after treatment with a dose of sodium salicylate (200 mg/kg) that reliably induces tinnitus in rats. Noise-burst detection thresholds were also measured to document the amount of hearing loss and aid in interpreting the gap detection results. As in the previous human psychophysical experiments, salicylate had little or no effect on gap thresholds measured in broadband noise presented at high-stimulus levels (30-60 dB SPL); gap detection thresholds were always 10 ms or less. Salicylate also did not affect gap thresholds presented in narrowband noise at 60 dB SPL. Therefore, rats treated with a dose of salicylate that reliably induces tinnitus have no difficulty detecting silent gaps as long as the noise in which they are embedded is clearly audible.

4.
Front Neurol ; 5: 179, 2014.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25278931

ABSTRACT

The phantom perception of tinnitus and reduced sound-level tolerance associated with hyperacusis have a high comorbidity and can be debilitating conditions for which there are no widely accepted treatments. One factor limiting the development of treatments for tinnitus and hyperacusis is the lack of reliable animal behavioral models of these disorders. Therefore, the purpose of this review is to highlight the current animal models of tinnitus and hyperacusis, and to detail the advantages and disadvantages of each paradigm. To date, this is the first review to include models of both tinnitus and hyperacusis.

5.
Neural Plast ; 2014: 658741, 2014.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24891959

ABSTRACT

Previous studies have shown that sodium salicylate (SS) activates not only central auditory structures, but also nonauditory regions associated with emotion and memory. To identify electrophysiological changes in the nonauditory regions, we recorded sound-evoked local field potentials and multiunit discharges from the striatum, amygdala, hippocampus, and cingulate cortex after SS-treatment. The SS-treatment produced behavioral evidence of tinnitus and hyperacusis. Physiologically, the treatment significantly enhanced sound-evoked neural activity in the striatum, amygdala, and hippocampus, but not in the cingulate. The enhanced sound evoked response could be linked to the hyperacusis-like behavior. Further analysis showed that the enhancement of sound-evoked activity occurred predominantly at the midfrequencies, likely reflecting shifts of neurons towards the midfrequency range after SS-treatment as observed in our previous studies in the auditory cortex and amygdala. The increased number of midfrequency neurons would lead to a relative higher number of total spontaneous discharges in the midfrequency region, even though the mean discharge rate of each neuron may not increase. The tonotopical overactivity in the midfrequency region in quiet may potentially lead to tonal sensation of midfrequency (the tinnitus). The neural changes in the amygdala and hippocampus may also contribute to the negative effect that patients associate with their tinnitus.


Subject(s)
Auditory Perceptual Disorders/chemically induced , Auditory Perceptual Disorders/physiopathology , Neuronal Plasticity/drug effects , Neuronal Plasticity/physiology , Salicylates , Acoustic Stimulation , Amygdala/physiology , Animals , Behavior, Animal/physiology , Electrodes, Implanted , Evoked Potentials, Auditory/drug effects , Gyrus Cinguli/physiology , Hippocampus/physiology , Loudness Perception/physiology , Male , Neostriatum/physiology , Rats , Rats, Sprague-Dawley , Reaction Time/physiology , Reflex, Startle/physiology , Tinnitus/physiopathology
6.
Behav Processes ; 106: 74-6, 2014 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24811450

ABSTRACT

Mice are emerging as an important behavioral model for studies of auditory perception and acoustic communication. These mammals frequently produce ultrasonic vocalizations, although the details of how these vocalizations are used for communication are not entirely understood. An important step in determining how they might be differentiating their calls is to measure discrimination and identification of the dimensions of various acoustic stimuli. Here, behavioral operant conditioning methods were employed to assess frequency difference limens for pure tones. We found that their thresholds were similar to those in other rodents but higher than in humans. We also asked mice, in an identification paradigm, whether they would use frequency or duration differences to classify stimuli varying on those two dimensions. We found that the mice classified the stimuli based on frequency rather than duration.


Subject(s)
Auditory Perception/physiology , Behavior, Animal/physiology , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Animals , Conditioning, Operant/physiology , Cues , Differential Threshold , Mice , Mice, Inbred CBA , Vocalization, Animal/physiology
7.
PLoS One ; 9(1): e85405, 2014.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24416405

ABSTRACT

The function of ultrasonic vocalizations (USVs) produced by mice (Mus musculus) is a topic of broad interest to many researchers. These USVs differ widely in spectrotemporal characteristics, suggesting different categories of vocalizations, although this has never been behaviorally demonstrated. Although electrophysiological studies indicate that neurons can discriminate among vocalizations at the level of the auditory midbrain, perceptual acuity for vocalizations has yet to be determined. Here, we trained CBA/CaJ mice using operant conditioning to discriminate between different vocalizations and between a spectrotemporally modified vocalization and its original version. Mice were able to discriminate between vocalization types and between manipulated vocalizations, with performance negatively correlating with spectrotemporal similarity. That is, discrimination performance was higher for dissimilar vocalizations and much lower for similar vocalizations. The behavioral data match previous neurophysiological results in the inferior colliculus (IC), using the same stimuli. These findings suggest that the different vocalizations could carry different meanings for the mice. Furthermore, the finding that behavioral discrimination matched neural discrimination in the IC suggests that the IC plays an important role in the perceptual discrimination of vocalizations.


Subject(s)
Auditory Perception/physiology , Inferior Colliculi/physiology , Vocalization, Animal/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation , Animals , Conditioning, Operant , Discrimination, Psychological , Female , Male , Mice , Mice, Inbred CBA , Neurons/physiology , Ultrasonics
8.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 129(5): 3384-92, 2011 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21568439

ABSTRACT

The present study examined auditory distance perception cues in a non-territorial songbird, the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata), and in a non-songbird, the budgerigar (Melopsittacus undulatus). Using operant conditioning procedures, three zebra finches and three budgerigars were trained to identify 1- (Near) and 75-m (Far) recordings of three budgerigar contact calls, one male zebra finch song, and one female zebra finch call. Once the birds were trained on these endpoint stimuli, other stimuli were introduced into the operant task. These stimuli included recordings at intermediate distances and artificially altered stimuli simulating changes in overall amplitude, high-frequency attenuation, reverberation, and all three cues combined. By examining distance cues (amplitude, high-frequency attenuation, and reverberation) separately, this study sought to determine which cue was the most salient for the birds. The results suggest that both species could scale the stimuli on a continuum from Near to Far and that amplitude was the most important cue for these birds in auditory distance perception, as in humans and other animals.


Subject(s)
Cues , Distance Perception/physiology , Finches/physiology , Melopsittacus/physiology , Sound Localization/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation , Animals , Conditioning, Operant/physiology , Female , Male , Sound Spectrography , Species Specificity , Vocalization, Animal
9.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19756650

ABSTRACT

Tone detection and temporal gap detection thresholds were determined in CBA/CaJ mice using a Go/No-go procedure and the psychophysical method of constant stimuli. In the first experiment, audiograms were constructed for five CBA/CaJ mice. Thresholds were obtained for eight pure tones ranging in frequency from 1 to 42 kHz. Audiograms showed peak sensitivity between 8 and 24 kHz, with higher thresholds at lower and higher frequencies. In the second experiment, thresholds for gap detection in broadband and narrowband noise bursts were measured at several sensation levels. For broadband noise, gap thresholds were between 1 and 2 ms, except at very low sensation levels, where thresholds increased significantly. Gap thresholds also increased significantly for low pass-filtered noise bursts with a cutoff frequency below 18 kHz. Our experiments revised absolute auditory thresholds in the CBA/CaJ mouse strain and demonstrated excellent gap detection ability in the mouse. These results add to the baseline behavioral data from normal-hearing mice which have become increasingly important for assessing auditory abilities in genetically altered mice.


Subject(s)
Auditory Threshold , Behavior, Animal , Motor Activity , Pattern Recognition, Physiological , Pitch Discrimination , Acoustic Stimulation , Animals , Audiometry, Pure-Tone , Conditioning, Operant , Cues , Discrimination, Psychological , Female , Male , Mice , Mice, Inbred CBA , Sound Spectrography , Time Perception
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...