Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 19 de 19
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Psychol Med ; 48(1): 168-174, 2018 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28874209

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Major depression and anxiety disorders are known to negatively influence cognitive performance. Moreover, there is evidence for greater cognitive decline in older adults with generalized anxiety disorder. Except for clinical studies, complex executive planning functions and subclinical levels of anxiety have not been examined in a population-based sample with a broad age range. METHODS: Planning performance was assessed using the Tower of London task in a population-based sample of 4240 participants aged 40-80 years from the Gutenberg Health Study (GHS) and related to self-reported anxiety and depression by means of multiple linear regression analysis. RESULTS: Higher anxiety ratings were associated with lower planning performance (ß = -0.20; p < 0.0001) independent of age (ß = 0.03; p = 0.47). When directly comparing the predictive value of depression and anxiety on cognition, only anxiety attained significance (ß = -0.19; p = 0.0047), whereas depression did not (ß = -0.01; p = 0.71). CONCLUSIONS: Subclinical levels of anxiety but not of depression showed negative associations with cognitive functioning independent of age. Our results demonstrate that associations observed in clinical groups might differ from those in population-based samples, also with regard to the trajectory across the life span. Further studies are needed to uncover causal interrelations of anxiety and cognition, which have been proposed in the literature, in order to develop interventions aimed at reducing this negative affective state and to improve executive functioning.


Subject(s)
Anxiety/complications , Anxiety/psychology , Cognitive Dysfunction/physiopathology , Neuropsychological Tests , Aged , Cognition , Cognitive Dysfunction/etiology , Cross-Sectional Studies , Depression/psychology , Executive Function , Female , Germany , Humans , Linear Models , Male , Middle Aged , Problem Solving , Prospective Studies , Psychomotor Performance
2.
Int J Cardiol ; 214: 256-61, 2016 Jul 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27077545

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: Distressed ('Type D') personality is associated with adverse health outcomes in patients with cardiovascular disease (CVD). While personality traits from the Five-Factor Model are related to cognitive functioning, neither Type D personality nor its underlying traits negative affectivity (NA) and social inhibition (SI) have been investigated regarding cognition. We therefore compared the predictive value of Type D classification and its subcomponents NA and SI on planning performance in individuals with and without CVD. METHODS: Type D personality traits (DS14) were determined in a population-based sample of 4026 participants (including 549 with CVD) aged 40-80years from the Gutenberg Health Study (GHS) and related to planning performance as assessed with the Tower of London task. Current depression and anxiety were controlled as state variables. RESULTS: Type D personality status was negatively associated with planning performance in the CVD patient group only (p<0.001) but had no impact in the non-CVD group (p=0.40). In the overall sample, NA was negatively and SI positively associated with planning performance. No differential effect on planning between groups was found for depression and anxiety. CONCLUSION: While the subcomponents NA and SI in the population-based sample confirm and extend previous research on personality traits and cognition, Type D personality classification in combination with CVD emerged as a risk factor for decreased cognitive functioning, independent of depression and anxiety. These findings implicate the need to early focus on individual differences in cognitive functioning in patients with CVD.


Subject(s)
Cardiovascular Diseases/psychology , Cognition/physiology , Type D Personality , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Cross-Sectional Studies , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Predictive Value of Tests , Psychiatric Status Rating Scales , Risk Factors
3.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol ; 30(7): 816-27, 2008 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18608695

ABSTRACT

In a behavioral experiment on 60 healthy volunteers, the Tower of London was employed as a complex visuo-spatial planning task. After each trial, participants were asked how difficult they found the task and whether they thought their solution was optimal. Results showed that objective problem difficulty affected behavioral performance as well as subjectively experienced difficulty and performance certainty. This pattern was influenced by experimental manipulation of participants' knowledge of objective problem difficulty: For optimally solved problems, performance certainty remained at high levels if such knowledge was provided, and strongly declined in more difficult problems if it was withheld. For nonoptimally solved problems, subjects' ratings indicated awareness of errors when they were informed about objective problem difficulty; otherwise, performance certainty declined from intermediate to low levels in more difficult problems. No such interaction was observed with regard to ratings of subjective problem difficulty. Additional structural equation modeling revealed that subjective awareness of errors and processing conflicts can be considered as independent only for optimally solved trials in which the optimal solution was known to the participants. We conclude that participants' ratings of problem difficulty and performance certainty can be regarded as indicators of at least partly distinct processes of performance monitoring, and that studies of complex problem solving incorporating such subjective measures may enhance the empirical basis of current theories of executive functioning.


Subject(s)
Conflict, Psychological , Movement/physiology , Problem Solving/physiology , Adult , Cues , Female , Humans , Male , Models, Psychological , Neuropsychological Tests , Numerical Analysis, Computer-Assisted , Young Adult
4.
Appl Environ Microbiol ; 73(1): 320-6, 2007 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17098919

ABSTRACT

Anaerobic reductive dehalogenation by Dehalococcoides spp. is an ideal system for studying functional diversity of closely related strains of bacteria. In Dehalococcoides spp., reductive dehalogenases (RDases) are key respiratory enzymes involved in the anaerobic detoxification of halogenated compounds at contaminated sites globally. Although housekeeping genes sequenced from Dehalococcoides spp. are >85% identical at the amino acid level, different strains are capable of dehalogenating diverse ranges of compounds, depending largely on the suite of RDase genes that each strain harbors and expresses. We identified RDase proteins that corresponded to known functions in four characterized cultures and predicted functions in an uncharacterized Dehalococcoides-containing mixed culture. Homologues within RDase subclusters containing PceA, TceA, and VcrA were among the most frequently identified proteins. Several additional proteins, including a formate dehydrogenase-like protein (Fdh), had high coverage in all strains and under all growth conditions.


Subject(s)
Chloroflexi/classification , Chloroflexi/enzymology , Oxidoreductases/metabolism , Peptides/chemistry , Peptides/metabolism , Proteomics , Amino Acid Sequence , Bacterial Proteins/metabolism , Chloroflexi/growth & development , Chromatography, Liquid , Culture Media , Formate Dehydrogenases/chemistry , Formate Dehydrogenases/metabolism , Molecular Sequence Data , Oxidoreductases/chemistry , Phylogeny , Species Specificity , Structure-Activity Relationship , Tandem Mass Spectrometry
5.
Br J Psychol ; 97(Pt 3): 299-311, 2006 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16848944

ABSTRACT

Playing chess requires problem-solving capacities in order to search through the chess problem space in an effective manner. Chess should thus require planning abilities for calculating many moves ahead. Therefore, we asked whether chess players are better problem solvers than non-chess players in a complex planning task. We compared planning performance between chess ( N=25) and non-chess players ( N=25) using a standard psychometric planning task, the Tower of London (ToL) test. We also assessed fluid intelligence (Raven Test), as well as verbal and visuospatial working memory. As expected, chess players showed better planning performance than non-chess players, an effect most strongly expressed in difficult problems. On the other hand, they showed longer planning and movement execution times, especially for incorrectly solved trials. No differences in fluid intelligence and verbal/visuospatial working memory were found between both groups. These findings indicate that better performance in chess players is associated with disproportionally longer solution times, although it remains to be investigated whether motivational or strategic differences account for this result.


Subject(s)
Aptitude , Games, Experimental , Play and Playthings , Problem Solving , Adult , Female , Humans , Intelligence , Male , Memory, Short-Term , Motivation , Orientation , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Reaction Time , Verbal Learning
6.
Neuroimage ; 30(2): 656-67, 2006 Apr 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16256375

ABSTRACT

Situationally adaptive behavior relies on the identification of relevant target stimuli, the evaluation of these with respect to the current context and the selection of an appropriate action. We used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to disentangle the neural networks underlying these processes within a single task. Our results show that activation of mid-ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (PFC) reflects the perceived presence of a target stimulus regardless of context, whereas context-appropriate evaluation is subserved by mid-dorsolateral PFC. Enhancing demands on response selection by means of response conflict activated a network of regions, all of which are directly connected to motor areas. On the midline, rostral anterior paracingulate cortex was found to link target detection and response selection by monitoring for the presence of behaviorally significant conditions. In summary, we provide new evidence for process-specific functional dissociations in the frontal lobes. In target-centered processing, target detection in the VLPFC is separable from contextual evaluation in the DLPFC. Response-centered processing in motor-associated regions occurs partly in parallel to these processes, which may enhance behavioral efficiency, but it may also lead to reaction time increases when an irrelevant response tendency is elicited.


Subject(s)
Decision Making/physiology , Frontal Lobe/physiology , Adult , Color Perception/physiology , Conflict, Psychological , Data Interpretation, Statistical , Discrimination, Psychological , Female , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Photic Stimulation , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology
7.
Brain Res Cogn Brain Res ; 23(2-3): 418-28, 2005 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15820648

ABSTRACT

Since the implementation of the Tower of London (ToL) test by Shallice in 1982, numerous variants differing in the tower's physical appearance have been developed. Here we compare behavioral performance (n = 31) on the original Tower of London task consisting of three rods of unequal lengths with a three-ball version of the Ward and Allport Tower Task (1997) using three equally sized rods. In the problem set used, the start and goal states for both tower configurations were identical across all trials. The experiment was divided into two parts: in the first block, the problems presented were equalized with respect to the number of paths for achieving an optimal solution, the minimum number of moves, goal hierarchy, subgoaling patterns, and suboptimal alternatives between the two tower versions. As expected, participants showed the same performance scores for both types of towers when structural problem parameters were identical. In the second block, participants had to solve five-move problems which-due to the different rod sizes of the towers-had only one optimal solution in the original version, but two optimal solutions in the variant with three rods of equal length. Participants revealed lower performance scores and showed longer planning times in the original version than in the second tower version. These findings demonstrate that the two tower versions are only interchangeable when specific planning parameters are equalized. Otherwise, even if problems look identical, significant differences in performance may be found due to the differing problem spaces in the two tower versions.


Subject(s)
Cognition/physiology , Frontal Lobe/physiology , Neuropsychological Tests , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Psychomotor Performance , Reaction Time
8.
Neuroimage ; 24(2): 586-90, 2005 Jan 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15627602

ABSTRACT

Several studies have attempted to identify the neuronal basis of sex differences in cognition. However, group differences in cognitive ability rather than genuine neurocognitive differences between the sexes may account for their results. Here, we compare with functional magnetic resonance imaging the relation between gender, individual task performance, and planning-related brain activation. Men and women preselected to display identical performance scores showed a strong relation between individual task performance and activation of the right dorsolateral prefrontal and right inferior parietal cortex activation during a visuospatial planning task. No gender-specific activations were found. However, a different pattern emerged when subjects had to execute the motor responses to the problems. Better performance was associated with right dorsolateral prefrontal and right parahippocampal activations, and females exhibited a stronger right hippocampal activation than males. These findings underline that an individual's performance level rather than his or her sex largely determines the neuronal activation patterns during higher-level cognition.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping/methods , Cognition/physiology , Neurons/physiology , Problem Solving , Adult , Brain/anatomy & histology , Brain/physiology , Female , Functional Laterality , Humans , Learning/physiology , Male , Sex Characteristics
9.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol ; 26(6): 846-56, 2004 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15370380

ABSTRACT

The Tower of London (ToL) test is widely used for measuring planning and aspects of problem solving. The primary focus of this study was to asses the relationship among different measures on the ToL. A secondary purpose was to examine the putative relationship between intelligence and working memory with that of ToL performance. Analyses of the interrelation of several ToL parameters indicated that better ToL performance was associated with longer preplanning time and shorter movement execution time. Good performers showed a stronger increase in preplanning duration with task difficulty then intermediate or poor planners. Stepwise multiple regression analysis yield fluid intelligence as the only significant predictor of ToL performance. These result suggest that the Tower of London assesses predominantly planning and problem solving and could not be explained by other cognitive domains.


Subject(s)
Cognition/physiology , Mental Processes/physiology , Neuropsychological Tests , Problem Solving/physiology , Weights and Measures , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Female , Humans , Intelligence/physiology , Male , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Reaction Time , Regression Analysis , Task Performance and Analysis , Time Factors , Verbal Learning/physiology
10.
Cereb Cortex ; 14(12): 1390-7, 2004 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15217897

ABSTRACT

The neuronal processes underlying correct and erroneous problem solving were studied in strong and weak problem-solvers using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). During planning, the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex was activated, and showed a linear relationship with the participants' performance level. A similar pattern emerged in right inferior parietal regions for all trials, and in anterior cingulate cortex for erroneously solved trials only. In the performance phase, when the pre-planned moves had to be executed by means of an fMRI-compatible computer mouse, the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex was again activated jointly with right parahippocampal cortex, and displayed a similar positive relationship with the participants' performance level. Incorrectly solved problems elicited stronger bilateral prefrontal and left inferior parietal activations than correctly solved trials. For both individual ability and trial-specific performance, our results thus demonstrate the crucial involvement of right prefrontal cortex in efficient visuospatial planning.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Individuality , Problem Solving/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Planning Techniques , Research Design
11.
Brain Res Cogn Brain Res ; 17(3): 675-83, 2003 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14561454

ABSTRACT

The Tower of London (ToL) is a well-known test of planning ability, and commonly used for the purpose of neuropsychological assessment and cognitive research. Its widespread application has led to numerous versions differing in a number of respects. The present study addressed the question whether differences in instruction, cueing, and learning processes systematically influence ToL performance across five difficulty levels (three to seven moves). A total of 81 normal adults were examined in a mixed design with the between-subject factor instruction (online versus mental preplanning) and the within-subject factors cueing (cue versus non-cue test version) and learning processes (first block and second block). We also assessed general intelligence for further analyses of differences between instruction groups. In general, there was a significant main effect across the difficulty levels indicating that the rate of incorrect solutions increased with problem difficulty. The participants who were instructed to make full mental plans before beginning to execute movements (preplanning) solved significantly more problems than people who started immediately with task-related movements (online). As for the cueing conditions, participants with the minimum number of moves predetermined (cue) could solve more trials than people who were only instructed to solve the problems in as few moves as possible (non-cue). Participants generally increased performance in the second part of the test session. However, an interaction of presentation order of the cueing condition with learning indicated that people who started the tasks with the non-cue version showed significantly better performance in the following cue condition, while participants who started with the cue condition stayed at the same performance level for both versions. These findings suggest that instruction, cueing conditions, and learning processes are important determinants of ToL performance, and they stress the necessity of standardized application in research and clinical practice.


Subject(s)
Cues , Learning/physiology , Neuropsychological Tests , Adult , Female , Humans , Intelligence Tests , Male , Psychomotor Performance/physiology
12.
Surg Neurol ; 45(4): 320-3, 1996 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8607078

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: The natural history of intracranial meningioma is no different in the Arabian peninsula than in other countries. The extent of the total loss of vision in one or both eyes, however, is unique to this area. METHODS: A retrospective study was conducted at King Faisal Specialist Hospital and Research Centre, a tertiary care institution in Saudi Arabia, on all cases of intracranial meningioma seen over a period of 9 years. The following data were collated: clinical presentation, site and size of tumor, radiologic features, surgical methods, and preoperative and postoperative visual status. RESULTS: Seventy of 232 cases (30.1%) of intracranial meningioma presented in the suprasellar area; the majority of these were more than 5 cm in diameter. Because of the large size it was not always possible to identify the exact point of origin or to achieve complete tumor removal. Deteriorating vision was the cardinal presenting feature and the incidence of blindness in one or both eyes at presentation was high (42.2%); 12.8% were totally blind on admission. None of these patients showed improvement after surgery (complete or subtotal removal of tumor). CONCLUSION: Lack of understanding of the symptoms and cultural influences are direct contributing factors to this complication of a benign intracranial lesion. This represents a serious public health concern in the Arabian peninsula.


Subject(s)
Blindness/etiology , Brain Neoplasms/complications , Meningioma/complications , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Blindness/epidemiology , Brain Neoplasms/surgery , Female , Humans , Male , Meningioma/surgery , Middle Aged , Optic Atrophy/epidemiology , Optic Atrophy/etiology , Retrospective Studies , Saudi Arabia/epidemiology , Sella Turcica , Treatment Outcome
13.
Childs Nerv Syst ; 10(7): 430-2, 1994 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7842432

ABSTRACT

Thirty-four patients with brain tumors were diagnosed and treated during the first 2 years of life, representing 8% of the 273 children treated for primary brain tumors from 1981 to 1990 inclusive. Large head circumference, vomiting, and altered level of consciousness were the chief findings at the time of presentation. Overall, the tumors were located mainly supratentorially; in patients in the 1st year of life they were equally distributed above and below the tentorium, while two-thirds of the patients presenting in the 2nd year of life had supratentorial tumors. The most common histological types were astrocytomas and medulloblastomas. Treatment methods were ventricular peritoneal shunt, craniotomy with total or partial removal of the tumor, adjuvant radiotherapy, and in a few cases chemotherapy. The 2-month mortality rate was 5.9%. The follow-up period ranged from 3 to 50 months.


Subject(s)
Brain Neoplasms/epidemiology , Brain Neoplasms/pathology , Brain Neoplasms/surgery , Child, Preschool , Craniotomy , Female , Humans , Incidence , Infant , Male , Saudi Arabia/epidemiology , Ventriculoperitoneal Shunt
14.
J Neurosurg ; 81(1): 126-9, 1994 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8207514

ABSTRACT

The case is presented of a prepontine arachnoid cyst in a 28-year-old woman. She was treated by craniotomy, stereotactic aspiration, internal shunting, and finally by insertion of a cystoperitoneal shunt. Eight months later she developed an asymptomatic Arnold-Chiari type I malformation. The pathogenesis of this unusual hindbrain herniation is explained on the basis of the differential pressure between two compartments and cephalocranial disproportion.


Subject(s)
Arachnoid Cysts/surgery , Encephalocele/etiology , Rhombencephalon/pathology , Ventriculoperitoneal Shunt/adverse effects , Adult , Female , Humans
15.
Surg Neurol ; 39(6): 443-50, 1993 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8516741

ABSTRACT

We report our experience with brain tumors in pediatric patients, patients who came to our attention when they were aged 14 years or less. In general, the pattern of brain tumors at this Institution is similar to that reported in the neurosurgical literature. Fortunately we have modern diagnostic technology and modern operating instrumentation available to use for dealing with such lesions, which are often in their late stages.


Subject(s)
Brain Neoplasms , Adolescent , Age Factors , Brain Neoplasms/diagnosis , Brain Neoplasms/epidemiology , Child , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Incidence , Infant , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle East/epidemiology , Time Factors , Tomography, X-Ray Computed
16.
Neurosurgery ; 28(3): 405-9, 1991 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1901395

ABSTRACT

In developing countries, 5 to 8% of the space-occupying lesions of the central nervous system are tuberculomas. Diagnosis can be difficult in the absence of extracranial tuberculosis; computed tomography is suggestive only. To assess the value of brain biopsies in tuberculomas, the records of 15 patients aged 6 to 80 years were reviewed. Histological confirmation was obtained in 15 patients, and acid-fast bacilli were cultured from 12 patients. Intracranial hypertension was the principal sign in 11 patients; other neurological signs were related to the location of the tuberculoma. One patient had evidence of extracranial tuberculosis. Biopsy-related complications consisted of an epidural hematoma in 1 patient and hydrocephalus in another; both required additional surgery. One case of tuberculous meningitis was probably related to surgery and poor drug compliance. There was no postoperative mortality. Thirteen patients (2 were lost to follow-up) were cured after an average of 16 months of antituberculous therapy. It was concluded that the brain biopsy is useful in diagnosing tuberculoma but that there is some associated risk.


Subject(s)
Brain Diseases/pathology , Tuberculoma/pathology , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Biopsy/adverse effects , Brain Diseases/complications , Brain Diseases/diagnostic imaging , Brain Diseases/microbiology , Child , Female , Follow-Up Studies , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Mycobacterium tuberculosis/isolation & purification , Reoperation , Retrospective Studies , Tomography, X-Ray Computed , Tuberculoma/complications , Tuberculoma/diagnostic imaging , Tuberculoma/microbiology , Tuberculosis, Meningeal/etiology
18.
Childs Nerv Syst ; 6(6): 368-9, 1990 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2257554

ABSTRACT

A rare case of intracranial teratoma is reported in which a tooth-like structure was clearly defined in the pituitary fossa. The authors are unaware of any similar case having been reported previously.


Subject(s)
Calcinosis/pathology , Pituitary Neoplasms/pathology , Teratoma/pathology , Calcinosis/diagnosis , Child , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Pituitary Neoplasms/diagnosis , Teratoma/diagnosis , Tomography, X-Ray Computed
19.
Article in German | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1983583

ABSTRACT

There is an increasing frequency of acute ischemic occlusion due to arterial thrombosis; the incidence has a range from 17% to 60%. In contrast to embolism we can observe a prolonged therapy less interval due to a lesser dramatic clinical course; in thrombotic occlusion only 10% can be treated within 6 h after onset of symptoms. In 11 to 93% of all cases extensive and comprehensive surgical techniques have to be performed for limb salvage. Nevertheless this aim only can be achieved in 60-84%.


Subject(s)
Embolism/surgery , Ischemia/surgery , Leg/blood supply , Thrombosis/surgery , Diagnosis, Differential , Embolism/diagnostic imaging , Humans , Ischemia/diagnostic imaging , Prognosis , Radiography , Thrombosis/diagnostic imaging
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...