Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 21
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Neurosci Lett ; 408(1): 16-20, 2006 Nov 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16982145

ABSTRACT

The present study investigated a cognitive aspect upon spatial perception, namely the impact of a true or false verbal feedback (FB) about the magnitude of body tilt on Subjective Proprioceptive Horizon (SPH) estimates. Subjects were asked to set their extended arm normal to gravity for different pitch body tilts up to 9 degrees . True FB were provided at all body tilt angles, whereas false FB were provided only at 6 degrees backward and 6 degrees forward body tilts for half of the trials. Our data confirmed previous results about the egocentric influence of body tilt itself upon SPH: estimates were linearly lowered with forward tilts and elevated with backward tilts. In addition, results showed a significant effect of the nature of the external FB provided to the subjects. When subjects received a false FB inducing a 3 degrees forward bias relative to physical body tilt, they set their SPH consequently higher than when they received a false FB inducing a 3 degrees backward bias. These findings clearly indicated that false cognitive information about body tilt might significantly modify the judgement of a geocentric direction of space, such as the SPH. This may have deleterious repercussions in aeronautics when pilots have to localize external objects relative to earth-based directions in darkened environments.


Subject(s)
Feedback , Orientation , Proprioception/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Adult , Female , Human Body , Humans , Male , Random Allocation
2.
Q J Exp Psychol A ; 54(2): 513-26, 2001 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11394059

ABSTRACT

This study investigates whether the vertical orientation may be predominantly used as an amodal reference norm by the visual, haptic, and somato-vestibular perceptual systems to define oblique orientations. We examined this question by asking the same sighted adult subjects to reproduce, in the frontal (roll) plane, the vertical (0 degree) and six oblique orientations in three tasks involving different perceptual systems. In the visual task, the subjects adjusted a moveable rod so that it reproduced the orientation of a visual rod seen previously in a dark room. In the haptic task, the blindfolded sighted subjects scanned an oriented rod with one hand and reproduced its orientation, with the same hand, on a moveable response rod. In the somato-vestibular task, the blind-folded sighted subjects, sitting in a rotating chair, adjusted this chair in order to reproduce the tested orientation of their own body. The results showed that similar oblique effects (unsigned angular error difference between six oblique orientations and vertical orientation) were observed across the three tasks. However, there were no positive correlations between the visual, haptic, and somato-vestibular oblique effects. Moreover, in some oblique orientations, there was a tendency to overestimate the angle between the oblique orientation and the vertical orientation. This effect varied according to the orientation value and the modality. Taken together, these findings suggest that although vertical orientation is used as a reference norm in the visual, haptic, and somato-vestibular systems to define oblique orientations, specific processing mechanisms seem to be at work in each perceptual system.


Subject(s)
Judgment/physiology , Kinesthesis/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Touch/physiology , Vestibule, Labyrinth/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Cues , Environment , Gravitation , Humans , Male , Periodicity
3.
Exp Brain Res ; 138(1): 62-70, 2001 May 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11374084

ABSTRACT

Seated observers requested to detect low-velocity passive rotations show a high motion-detection threshold. However, when standing on a slowly rotating platform, their equilibrium is preserved, suggesting that cognitive sensing and sensorimotor reactions do not share the same central processes. The present experiments investigated the ability of observers seated on a slowly rotating chair in total darkness to indicate with their hand the position of briefly flashed targets (Experiment 1) and to indicate the subjective horizon with an outstretched arm (Experiment 2) or with a target driven by a joystick (Experiment 3). The overall hypothesis stated that egocentric coding of the position of a target should not be affected by sensing or not-sensing body rotation (Experiment 1), while geocentric positioning may (Experiments 2 and 3). Our data partially supported the hypothesis. Subjects pointed accurately to the memorized targets (Experiment 1), whereas misperception of body orientation was a source of inaccuracy for actions referred to a geocentric frame (Experiments 2 and 3). More interestingly, subjects' perceptions changed as a single, smooth, and monotonic function of tilt, independent of whether the perception of body orientation was present or not.


Subject(s)
Motor Activity/physiology , Movement/physiology , Orientation/physiology , Posture , Proprioception/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Reference Values , Regression Analysis , Rotation
4.
Percept Psychophys ; 63(1): 36-46, 2001 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11304015

ABSTRACT

The visually perceived eye level (VPEL) has been shown to shift toward the lower part of the body in upright subjects facing toward the axis of rotation on a centrifuge. This shift occurs in the same direction as the shift in the gravito-inertial forces (Gis) produced by very low radial acceleration (centrifugation) combined with gravity. The purpose of this study was to determine whether the same phenomenon affects the visually perceived apparent zenith (VPAZ) in subjects in a supine position. Twelve supine subjects were instructed to set a luminous target to the VPAZ, either while they were in total darkness and motionless or while undergoing very low centrifugation. Data showed that Gis induced a VPAZ shift similar to that observed for the VPEL. Thus, as is the case for the VPEL, the corresponding logarithmic psychophysical function of the VPAZ may be considered to be a type of oculogravic illusion phenomenon with differences in the subjects' that differs from subject to subject, depending on the subject's sensitivity to low radial accelerations. Data on VPEL and VPAZ support the notion that the subjective perception of eye level in total darkness takes into account changes--even if extremely slight-in the direction of the gravito-inertial forces produced by the combination of gravity and low radial accelerations, although subjects are unaware of the Gi shift. However, depending on the intensity of the radial acceleration and the angular deviation of Gi relative to G, the shift of the VPEL and the VPAZ can be either amplified or attenuated. Moreover, differences between VPEL and VPAZ responses suggest two explanatory assumptions--namely, that this is (1) a peripheral phenomenon dependent on the neurophysiological anisotropy of the otolithic system or (2) a central phenomenon dependent on the relevance assigned to the peripheral information by the integrative sensory functions and the associative processes.


Subject(s)
Motion Perception , Supine Position , Adult , Gravitation , Humans , Male , Psychophysics , Random Allocation
5.
Int J Psychophysiol ; 42(3): 243-51, 2001 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11812391

ABSTRACT

This study investigated the effects of heat exposure, exercise-induced dehydration and fluid ingestion on cognitive performance. Seven healthy men, unacclimatized to heat, were kept euhydrated or were dehydrated by controlled passive exposure to heat (H, two sessions) or by treadmill exercise (E, two sessions) up to a weight loss of 2.8%. On completion of a 1-h recovery period, the subjects drank a solution containing 50 g l(-1) glucose and 1.34 g l(-1) NaCl in a volume of water corresponding to 100% of his body weight loss induced by dehydration. (H1 and E1) or levels of fluid deficit were maintained (H0, E0). In the E0, H0 and control conditions, the subject drank a solution containing the same quantity of glucose diluted in 100 ml of water. Psychological tests were administered 30 min after the dehydration phase and 2 h after fluid ingestion. Both dehydration conditions impaired cognitive abilities (i.e. perceptive discrimination, short-term memory), as well as subjective estimates of fatigue, without any relevant differences between the methods. By 3.5 h after fluid deficit, dehydration (H0 and E0) no longer had any adverse effect, although the subjects felt increasingly tired. Thus, there was no beneficial effect of fluid ingestion (H1 and E1) on the cognitive variables. However, long-term memory retrieval was impaired in both control and dehydration situations, whereas there was no decrement in performance in the fluid ingestion condition (H1, E1).


Subject(s)
Cognition/physiology , Dehydration/physiopathology , Drinking/physiology , Heat Stress Disorders/physiopathology , Physical Exertion/physiology , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Cognition/drug effects , Dehydration/psychology , Exercise/physiology , Exercise Test/drug effects , Exercise Test/psychology , Glucose/pharmacology , Heat Stress Disorders/psychology , Hot Temperature , Humans , Male , Rehydration Solutions/pharmacology
6.
Perception ; 30(12): 1427-38, 2001.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11817750

ABSTRACT

We compared the contribution and the effectiveness of modulating the orientation perception of two types of visual information: the visual frame and the visual polarity of objects. In experiment 1, we examined the effect of a square frame, a mouse, an elephant, and a map of France on the apparent vertical. In the upright position, despite the presence of tilted individual component features, the visual objects had no illusory visual tilt effects. When tilted, these objects had a substantial effect on the direction that appeared to be vertical. However, rod-setting errors were smaller in the inducing objects than when observed with the frame display. In the second experiment, the results of experiment 1 were replicated with a meaningful circular contour--a porthole and a clock. The presence of the external circular contour did not abolish the illusion on the apparent vertical. Moreover, in experiment 3, a clock whose numbers were displaced and not tilted--to avoid the possible tilt influence of visual cues--was also able to deflect the subjective visual vertical. This finding suggests that through top-down processing shapes can act as a framework which serves as a reference influencing the perceived orientation of the inner objects.


Subject(s)
Cognition/physiology , Orientation/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Female , Humans , Male
7.
Ergonomics ; 43(4): 474-85, 2000 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10801081

ABSTRACT

This study investigated the effects of hypoxia on parallel/preattentional and serial/attentional processes in early vision, and the use of a positive-end-expiratory-pressure (PEEP) to prevent the impairment in performance. Twenty-one subjects were submitted to an 8-h hypoxia exposure in a hypobaric chamber (4500 m, 589 hPa, 22 degrees C), both with and without a 5-cm H2O PEEP. Subjects carried out a visual search task consisting of detecting a target among distractors in normoxia, in acute and in prolonged hypoxia. Conjointly their sensitivity to acute mountain sickness (AMS) was scored through the Lake Louise AMS scoring system. Results showed that prolonged hypoxia slowed serial/attentional processing whereas parallel/preattentional processes were not impaired either by acute or by prolonged hypoxia. PEEP prevented serial/attentional processes from slowing and those effects were more clearly observed in the AMS sensitive subjects with respect to the AMS insensitive subjects. These results suggest that the slowing induced by prolonged hypoxia is specific to an early visual process that pilots the scanning of an attentional spotlight throughout the visual field.


Subject(s)
Altitude Sickness/physiopathology , Attention/physiology , Hypoxia/physiopathology , Mental Processes/physiology , Positive-Pressure Respiration , Adult , Altitude Sickness/complications , Humans , Hypoxia/complications , Male , Sensitivity and Specificity
8.
J Vestib Res ; 10(6): 251-8, 2000.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11455106

ABSTRACT

The purpose of this study was to investigate how experts in motor skills requiring a fine postural control perceive their body orientation with few gravity based sensory cues. In Experiment 1, expert gymnasts and controls had to detect their body tilt when pitching at a velocity of 0.05 deg.s(-1), in two conditions of body restriction (strapped and body cast altering the somatosensory cues). Contrary to the experts, the controls exhibited a larger body tilt when totally restrained in the body cast. In Experiment 2, subjects had to estimate their Subjective Postural Vertical (SPV) starting from different angles of pitch tilt. The controls exhibited significant errors of SPV judgement whereas the experts were very precise. These results suggest that 1) somatosensory cues are more informative than otolithic cues for the perception of body orientation, and 2) the efficiency of otolithic and/or interoceptive inputs can be improved through a specific training to compensate for the lack of somatosensory cues.


Subject(s)
Gymnastics/psychology , Orientation/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Cues , Darkness , Female , Gravitation , Humans , Male , Posture/physiology , Semicircular Canals/physiology , Touch/physiology
9.
Percept Psychophys ; 61(4): 615-24, 1999 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10370332

ABSTRACT

The purpose of the present experiment was to investigate the extent to which subjects can perceive, at very slow velocities, an angular rotation of the support surface about the medio-lateral axis of the ankle, knee, hip, or neck joint when visual cues are not available. Subjects were passively displaced on a slowly rotating platform at .01, .03, and .05 deg/sec. The subjects' task was to detect movements of the platform in four different postural conditions allowing body oscillations about the ankle, knee, hip, or neck joint. In Experiment 1, subjects had to detect backward and forward rotation (pitching). In Experiment 2, they had to detect left and right rotations of the platform (rolling). In Experiment 3, subjects had to detect both backward/forward and left/right rotations of the platform, with the body fixed and the head either fixed or free to move. Overall, when the body was free to oscillate about the ankle, knee, or hip joints, a similar threshold for movement perception was observed. This threshold was lower for rolling than for pitching. Interestingly, in these postural conditions, an unconscious compensation in the direction opposite to the platform rotation was observed on most trials. The threshold for movement perception was much higher when the head was the only segment free to oscillate about the neck joint. These results suggest that, in static conditions, the otoliths are poor detectors of the direction of gravity forces. They also suggest that accurate perception of body orientation is improved when proprioceptive information can be dynamically integrated.


Subject(s)
Differential Threshold/physiology , Gravity Sensing/physiology , Joints/physiology , Orientation/physiology , Proprioception/physiology , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Female , Humans , Leg , Male , Motion Perception/physiology , Neck , Posture/physiology , Rotation , Time and Motion Studies
10.
Brain Res Cogn Brain Res ; 6(3): 227-32, 1998 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9479074

ABSTRACT

In order to study the processes of central vision in deaf subjects, 12 congenitally deaf adults and 12 normally hearing adults performed a visual search task. The task consisted of detecting a "Q" target among "O" distractors in variable numbers and the reverse. The method used a paradigm based on the stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) specifically designed to measure the visual processing time between the visual array and a mask. A different visual search pattern was observed in each group. The hearing subjects showed an asymmetrical visual search pattern (parallel versus serial processing respectively for "Q" and "O" targets). In contrast, the deaf subjects showed a symmetrical search pattern (parallel processing in both experimental conditions). In a visual task selectively supported by central vision, visual processes of the congenitally deaf are more efficient when the task involves the contribution of serial processes.


Subject(s)
Deafness/congenital , Visual Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Case-Control Studies , Computers , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation , Reference Values
11.
Aviat Space Environ Med ; 68(11): 993-7, 1997 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9383498

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Exposure to high altitudes requires acclimation or acclimatization, to prevent the negative effects of severe hypoxia. Among several methods, short acclimation with intermittent exposure to severe hypoxia in a hypobaric chamber triggers efficient physiological pre-adaptation mechanisms (11-13). However, we have little knowledge about the cognitive repercussions of such an acclimation protocol. METHODS: Four mountaineers were tested daily in the course of a short acclimation protocol (5 d). After their SaO2 (arterial oxyhemoglogin saturation) were recorded, they carried out a choice reaction time task (Manikin test) twice every day; first at ground level (250 m, control sessions), second at the highest altitude of the day (D1 = 5000 m, D2 = 5500 m, D3 = 6000 m, D4 = 6500 m, D5 = 7000 m). RESULTS: High altitude SaO2 level decreased during the first 3 d, then stabilized around 72-73%. Despite a slight and transient increase at the highest altitude relative to the ground level in D4, the error rate remained low throughout the protocol. Further, response time to the Manikin task did not show significant changes among the days during the acute stage of hypoxia relative to ground level up to 7000 m. CONCLUSIONS: On the whole, it seems that a short acclimation protocol based on intermittent exposure to simulated high altitudes triggered adaptive processes without major impairment in a choice reaction time task during the acute stages of severe hypoxia.


Subject(s)
Acclimatization , Altitude Sickness/prevention & control , Cognition , Hypoxia/physiopathology , Acute Disease , Adult , Blood Gas Analysis , Humans , Hypoxia/blood , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Oxyhemoglobins/analysis , Psychomotor Performance , Reaction Time
12.
Percept Mot Skills ; 85(1): 43-50, 1997 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9293555

ABSTRACT

This experiment dealt with the role of strobe frequency on the rod and frame effect in frame-dependent and frame-independent subjects in light of the destabilizing effect of strobe lighting on body posture. Analysis showed that the frame effect was resistant to strobe illumination and was significantly stronger at 2 Hz than at 9 Hz. Since the stroboscopic effect was not related to the extent of the frame effect observed in normal light, there was no over-all dependence on the different components of the visual field (static and kinetic). Moreover, analysis of eye movements during stroboscopic exposure confirmed previous observation of a visual scanning style related to orienting activity.


Subject(s)
Light , Orientation , Posture , Space Perception , Visual Fields , Adult , Eye Movements , Female , Humans , Male , Periodicity , Photic Stimulation
13.
Percept Psychophys ; 58(6): 906-14, 1996 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8768185

ABSTRACT

The purpose of this study was to determine how the combined effects of a reference frame and of very low gravito-inertial forces produced by centrifugation affect the visually perceived eye level (VPEL). Twenty subjects were instructed to set a luminous target to the VPEL under various experimental conditions involving two main factors: (1) visual context (frameless, frame centered, frame moved down 50 mm, and frame moved up 50 mm) and (2) gravito-inertial context (motionless, Gi1 = 9.81001 m/sec2 and Gi2 = 9.95 m/sec2). The visual context significantly reduced the lowering of VPEL in darkness as caused by radial acceleration; this confirms the prevailing role of vision versus propriosomesthesis. However, under condition Gi2, there was a significant effect on the VPEL in spite of the presence of the luminous frame; this demonstrates that VPEL processing involves both visual and propriosomesthesic information. Furthermore, the VPEL varied linearly with the vertical shift of the luminous frame for any of the gravito-inertial conditions used in this study, but, under condition Gi2, the VPEL was shifted downward.


Subject(s)
Attention , Field Dependence-Independence , Kinesthesis , Orientation , Proprioception , Visual Perception , Acceleration , Adult , Dark Adaptation , Female , Gravitation , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Psychophysics , Rotation
14.
Perception ; 24(6): 623-30, 1995.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7478903

ABSTRACT

The visual angle subtended by the frame seems to be an important determinant of the contribution of orientation contrast and illusion of self-tilt (ie vection) to the rod-and-frame effect. Indeed, the visuovestibular factor (which produces vection) seems to be predominant in large displays and the contrast effect in small displays. To determine how these two phenomena are combined to account for the rod-and-frame effect, independent estimates of the magnitude of each component in relation to the angular size subtended by the display were examined. Thirty-five observers were exposed to three sets of experimental situations: body-adjustment test (illusion of self-tilt only), the tilt illusion (contrast only) and the rod-and-frame test, each display subtending 7, 12, 28, and 45 deg of visual angle. Results showed that errors recorded in the three situations increased linearly with the angular size. Whatever the size of the frame, both mechanisms, contrast effect (tilt illusion) and illusory effect on self-orientation (body-adjustment test), are always present. However, rod-and-frame errors became greater at a faster rate than the other two effects as the size of teh stimuli became larger. Neither one nor the other independent phenomenen, nor the combined effect could fully account for the rod-and-frame effect whatever the angular size of the apparatus.


Subject(s)
Attention , Contrast Sensitivity , Discrimination Learning , Kinesthesis , Optical Illusions , Orientation , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Postural Balance , Posture , Psychophysics
15.
Aviat Space Environ Med ; 65(3): 204-8, 1994 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8185548

ABSTRACT

The purpose of this study was to determine whether very low gravito-inertial forces produced by centrifugation affect the visually perceived eye level (VPEL) in the same way as the oculagravic illusion. Eleven subjects in total darkness were instructed to set a luminous target to the VPEL, either while they were motionless or undergoing very low centrifugation. Results showed a significant effect on VPEL at 0.01 m/s2 radial acceleration, which corresponds to a resultant gravito-inertial equal to 9.81001 m/s2. This radial acceleration value is lower than the lowest perception thresholds previously measured for a linear acceleration (about 0.05 m/s2). Thus, as previous results have shown that the oculogyral illusion indirectly decreases perceptual thresholds for the perception of angular acceleration in darkness, the lowering of the VPEL indirectly decreases thresholds for perception of radial acceleration produced by centrifugation. Moreover, there is a logarithmic relationship between very low centrifugation and the positioning of the VPEL at a lower level. This relationship is explained as a direct and sole effect on the sensory utricular otolithic membrane by the radial acceleration of centrifugation.


Subject(s)
Acceleration , Motion Perception , Visual Perception , Adult , Humans , Male , Sensory Thresholds
17.
J Am Vet Med Assoc ; 181(8): 808-10, 1982 Oct 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7141978

ABSTRACT

Of 122 horses with pleural effusion, 90 (73.8%) had pleuritis secondary to pneumonia or lung abscessation. Fifty-one horses died or were euthanatized. The highest prevalence was in Thoroughbred and Standardbred racehorses. Eleven (12.2%) horses were postsurgical patients and 22 (24.4%) horses had been transported over 500 miles. There was no relationship between final outcome and the age, sex, breed, hematologic values, or laboratory findings pertaining to pleural fluid except for the bacterial isolation of Escherichia coli from the pleural fluid, as this was more frequently associated with death. Follow-up on 38 of the 39 horses that survived showed that 18 (46.2%) recovered and were able to return to performance equal to that prior to their illness. Ten (25.6%) were returned for breeding or pleasure use, with no attempt made to return them to racing. Follow-up was not available for 5 horses, 4 horses had just recently been discharged from the hospital, and 2 horses are racing poorer than prior to their illness.


Subject(s)
Horse Diseases/etiology , Lung Abscess/veterinary , Pleurisy/veterinary , Pneumonia/veterinary , Animals , Female , Horses , Lung Abscess/complications , Male , Pleural Effusion/microbiology , Pleurisy/etiology , Pneumonia/complications
18.
J Am Vet Med Assoc ; 181(5): 470-3, 1982 Sep 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-6982261

ABSTRACT

A flexible fiberoptic endoscope was used to examine the upper respiratory tract of 479 horses and 41 (8.6%) had abnormalities. Pharyngeal lymphoid hyperplasia (PLH) was found in 141 horses (29.5%). Statistical analysis showed a relationship between the age of the horse and the prevalence of PLH. Sixteen (3.3%), 10 (2.1%), and 6 (1.3%), of the horses had laryngeal hemiplegia, epiglottic entrapment, and dorsal displacement of the soft palate, respectively. There was no association between the age of the horse and the prevalence of any of these abnormalities; nor was there a positive correlation between the prevalence of exercise-induced pulmonary hemorrhage and PLH or laryngeal hemiplegia.


Subject(s)
Horse Diseases/epidemiology , Respiratory Tract Diseases/veterinary , Age Factors , Animals , Cross-Sectional Studies , Cysts/epidemiology , Cysts/veterinary , Endoscopy/veterinary , Fiber Optic Technology , Hemorrhage/epidemiology , Hemorrhage/veterinary , Horse Diseases/diagnosis , Horses , Hyperplasia , Lung Diseases/epidemiology , Lung Diseases/veterinary , Lymphoid Tissue/pathology , Pennsylvania , Pharyngeal Diseases/epidemiology , Pharyngeal Diseases/veterinary , Pharynx/pathology , Physical Exertion , Respiratory Tract Diseases/diagnosis , Respiratory Tract Diseases/epidemiology
19.
Am J Vet Res ; 43(7): 1123-7, 1982 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7103190

ABSTRACT

Thoroughbred horses (n = 191) were examined with a flexible fiberoptic endoscope within 2 hours of racing on a dirt track; 147 (75.4%) had evidence of exercise-induced pulmonary hemorrhage (EIPH), and 13 (9.0%) had blood at the nostrils. Of 107 Thoroughbreds examined within the same period after breezing, 41 (38.3%) had evidence of EIPH. One horse (2.4%) of this group had blood at the nostrils. Statistical analysis of frequency data showed that a relationship existed between EIPH and the horse's age or distance raced or breezed. Relationship did not exist between EIPH and sex or finishing position. Thoroughbreds were also examined endoscopically after steeplechase, flat turf, and timber races; 67.7% (21/31), 14.3% (2/14), and 66.6% (2/3) of the horses in such races were EIPH-positive, respectively; and 14.3% (3/21), 0% (0/2), and 100% (2/2) of these EIPH-positive horses had blood at the nostrils. Of 32 breezing Thoroughbreds in a 3rd survey, 21 (65.5%) were EIPH-positive. None bled from the nostrils. Endoscopic findings of EIPH are repeatable in the horses, indicating that bleeding is not a random event.


Subject(s)
Hemorrhage/veterinary , Horse Diseases/etiology , Lung Diseases/veterinary , Physical Exertion , Age Factors , Animals , Endoscopy/veterinary , Female , Fiber Optic Technology , Hemorrhage/epidemiology , Hemorrhage/etiology , Horse Diseases/epidemiology , Horses , Lung Diseases/epidemiology , Lung Diseases/etiology , Male , Pennsylvania , Sex Factors
20.
J Am Vet Med Assoc ; 180(8): 874-7, 1982 Apr 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7085464

ABSTRACT

Three horses with brain abscesses had different clinical manifestations: 1 had a protracted clinical course whereas 2 had a short clinical course. Clinical signs in 2 horses (1 acute case, 1 chronic case) included unilateral loss of vision, head tilt, circling, abnormal mental status, and ataxia. The 3rd horse had bilateral loss of vision, altered mental status, and apparent deafness. Results of cerebrospinal fluid analysis were inconsistent. The horse with the protracted clinical course had paradoxic central vestibular disease.


Subject(s)
Brain Abscess/veterinary , Horse Diseases/physiopathology , Streptococcal Infections/veterinary , Animals , Brain/pathology , Brain Abscess/pathology , Brain Abscess/physiopathology , Female , Horse Diseases/pathology , Horses , Male , Streptococcal Infections/pathology , Streptococcal Infections/physiopathology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...