Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 6 de 6
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Type of study
Language
Publication year range
1.
Proc Biol Sci ; 290(1991): 20221496, 2023 01 25.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36651050

ABSTRACT

Attentional set shifting is a core ingredient of cognition, allowing for fast adaptation to changes in the environment. How this skill compares between humans and other primates is not well known. We examined performance of 3- to 5-year-old children and chimpanzees on a new attentional set shifting task. We presented participants with two shelves holding the same set of four boxes. To choose the correct box on each shelf, one has to switch attention depending on which shelf one is currently presented with. Experiment 1 (forty-six 3- to 5-year olds, predominantly European White) established content validity, showing that the majority of errors were specific switching mistakes indicating failure to shift attention. Experiment 2 (one hundred and seventy-eight 3- to 6-year olds, predominantly European White) showed that older children made fewer mistakes, but if mistakes were made, a larger proportion were switching mistakes rather than 'random' errors. Experiment 3 (52 chimpanzees) established suitability of the task for non-human great apes and showed that chimpanzees' performance was comparable to the performance of 3- and 4-year olds, but worse than 5-year olds. These results suggest that chimpanzees and young children share attentional set shifting capacities, but that there are unique changes in the human lineage from 5 years of age.


Subject(s)
Attention , Pan troglodytes , Animals , Humans , Child, Preschool , Child , Adolescent , Cognition , Reversal Learning
2.
Evol Hum Sci ; 4: e5, 2022.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37588934

ABSTRACT

Associative Tool Use (ATU) describes the use of two or more tools in combination, with the literature further differentiating between Tool set use, Tool composite use, Sequential tool use and Secondary tool use. Research investigating the cognitive processes underlying ATU has shown that some primate and bird species spontaneously invent Tool set and Sequential tool use. Yet studies with humans are sparse. Whether children are also able to spontaneously invent ATU behaviours and at what age this ability emerges is poorly understood. We addressed this gap in the literature with two experiments involving preschoolers (E1, N = 66, 3 years 6 months to 4 years 9 months; E2, N = 119, 3 years 0 months to 6 years 10 months) who were administered novel tasks measuring Tool set, Metatool and Sequential tool use. Participants needed to solve the tasks individually, without the opportunity for social learning (except for enhancement effects). Children from 3 years of age spontaneously invented all of the types of investigated ATU behaviours. Success rates were low, suggesting that individual invention of ATU in novel tasks is still challenging for preschoolers. We discuss how future studies can use and expand our tasks to deepen our understanding of tool use and problem-solving in humans and non-human animals.

3.
Sci Rep ; 7(1): 1788, 2017 05 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28496154

ABSTRACT

The ratchet effect - the accumulation of beneficial changes in cultural products beyond a level that individuals could reach on their own - is a topic of increasing interest. It is currently debated which social learning mechanisms allow for the generation and transmission of cumulative culture. This study focused on transmission, investigating whether 4- to 6-year-old children were able to copy cumulative technological design and whether they could do so without action information (emulation). We adapted the spaghetti tower task, previously used to test for accumulation of culture in human adults. A baseline condition established that the demonstrated tower design was beyond the innovation skills of individual children this age and so represented a culture-dependent product for them. There were 2 demonstration conditions: a full demonstration (actions plus (end-)results) and an endstate- demonstration (end-results only). Children in both demonstration conditions built taller towers than those in the baseline. Crucially, in both demonstration conditions some children also copied the demonstrated tower. We provide the first evidence that young children learn from, and that some of them even copy, cumulative technological design, and that - in line with some adult studies - action information is not always necessary to transmit culture-dependent traits.


Subject(s)
Culture , Imitative Behavior , Social Behavior , Age Factors , Child , Child, Preschool , Creativity , Female , Humans , Male , Technology
4.
Proc Biol Sci ; 283(1825): 20152402, 2016 Feb 24.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26911964

ABSTRACT

The variety and complexity of human-made tools are unique in the animal kingdom. Research investigating why human tool use is special has focused on the role of social learning: while non-human great apes acquire tool-use behaviours mostly by individual (re-)inventions, modern humans use imitation and teaching to accumulate innovations over time. However, little is known about tool-use behaviours that humans can invent individually, i.e. without cultural knowledge. We presented 2- to 3.5-year-old children with 12 problem-solving tasks based on tool-use behaviours shown by great apes. Spontaneous tool use was observed in 11 tasks. Additionally, tasks which occurred more frequently in wild great apes were also solved more frequently by human children. Our results demonstrate great similarity in the spontaneous tool-use abilities of human children and great apes, indicating that the physical cognition underlying tool use shows large overlaps across the great ape species. This suggests that humans are neither born with special physical cognition skills, nor that these skills have degraded due to our species' long reliance of social learning in the tool-use domain.


Subject(s)
Cognition , Pan troglodytes/physiology , Pongo/physiology , Problem Solving , Tool Use Behavior , Animals , Child, Preschool , Female , Germany , Humans , Learning , Male , Play and Playthings , United Kingdom
5.
Diabetes Care ; 7(6): 548-56, 1984.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-6510180

ABSTRACT

This study compares the utility of nonenzymatically glycosylated serum proteins (lys-GSP) to glycosylated hemoglobins (HbA1a-c) as control indices of glucose homeostasis in patients with IDDM. The diagnostic value of lys-GSP was also examined in patients with non-insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus, in subjects with impaired glucose tolerance, and in two patients with insulinoma. The intraindividual fluctuation of lys-GSP in normoglycemic subjects is very small, resulting in an interindividual range of 3.0 +/- 0.3 lysine-bound glucose/mg protein (means +/- SD, N = 52). HbA1a-c with a normal range of 6.4 +/- 0.9% (N = 52) shows greater variability. In IDDM there is no overlap of lys-GSP levels between the normal and the diabetic range at the 95% confidence level. In patients treated with an open-loop insulin delivery system failure of normalization of the glucose balance was clearly discernible by an elevation of GSP. In contrast, in about 40% of the patients with incomplete glycemic control the HbA1a-c levels fell within the normal range. The utility of lys-GSP for diagnosis of diabetes is compared with the results of 60 oral glucose tolerance tests. Two patients suffering from insulinoma displayed decreased lys-GSP values. From these results it appears that determination of lys-GSP represents a more sensitive parameter for long-term control than HbA1a-c and is suitable for monitoring even small fluctuations of blood glucose.


Subject(s)
Blood Glucose/metabolism , Blood Proteins/metabolism , Diabetes Mellitus, Type 1/blood , Diabetes Mellitus, Type 2/blood , Glycated Hemoglobin/metabolism , Glycoproteins , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Child , Chromatography, High Pressure Liquid , Female , Glucose Tolerance Test , Glycation End Products, Advanced , Humans , Insulinoma/blood , Male , Middle Aged , Pancreatic Neoplasms/blood , Serum Albumin/metabolism , Glycated Serum Proteins , Glycated Serum Albumin
6.
Food Chem Toxicol ; 21(6): 785-9, 1983 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-6686583

ABSTRACT

Ten volunteers (males and females) each had their hair dyed 13 times at intervals of 3-6 wk. Each volunteer used a single commercial preparation throughout the study. The preparations used contained a mixture of aminotoluenes, aminophenols and hydroxybenzenes and, in some cases, naphthol, as the active ingredients. Lymphocytes of the hair-dyed volunteers and of ten controls matched for age and sex were scored for chromosomal aberrations. The incidence of aberrations did not differ significantly between the controls and the hair-dyed volunteers at any of the nine sampling times (before the first exposure, after the first (sham) dyeing and then after each of the next three and the last four dyeing procedures). An increase in the aberration rate with time was observed both in the controls and in the hair-dyed subjects. The reason for this increase could not be determined. No clastogenic effect of repeated hair dyeing was established in this study.


Subject(s)
Chromosome Aberrations , Hair Dyes/adverse effects , Hair Preparations/adverse effects , Adult , Aging , Female , Hair Dyes/blood , Humans , Male
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...