Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 7 de 7
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Vision Res ; 40(10-12): 1469-87, 2000.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10788653

ABSTRACT

Large changes in a scene often become difficult to notice if made during an eye movement, image flicker, movie cut, or other such disturbance. It is argued here that this change blindness can serve as a useful tool to explore various aspects of vision. This argument centers around the proposal that focused attention is needed for the explicit perception of change. Given this, the study of change perception can provide a useful way to determine the nature of visual attention, and to cast new light on the way that it is - and is not - involved in visual perception. To illustrate the power of this approach, this paper surveys its use in exploring three different aspects of vision. The first concerns the general nature of seeing. To explain why change blindness can be easily induced in experiments but apparently not in everyday life, it is proposed that perception involves a virtual representation, where object representations do not accumulate, but are formed as needed. An architecture containing both attentional and nonattentional streams is proposed as a way to implement this scheme. The second aspect concerns the ability of observers to detect change even when they have no visual experience of it. This sensing is found to take on at least two forms: detection without visual experience (but still with conscious awareness), and detection without any awareness at all. It is proposed that these are both due to the operation of a nonattentional visual stream. The final aspect considered is the nature of visual attention itself - the mechanisms involved when scrutinizing items. Experiments using controlled stimuli show the existence of various limits on visual search for change. It is shown that these limits provide a powerful means to map out the attentional mechanisms involved.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Eye Movements/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Humans , Memory/physiology , Models, Psychological , Terminology as Topic
2.
J Exp Psychol Gen ; 129(4): 481-507, 2000 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11142864

ABSTRACT

Advances in neuroscience implicate reentrant signaling as the predominant form of communication between brain areas. This principle was used in a series of masking experiments that defy explanation by feed-forward theories. The masking occurs when a brief display of target plus mask is continued with the mask alone. Two masking processes were found: an early process affected by physical factors such as adapting luminance and a later process affected by attentional factors such as set size. This later process is called masking by object substitution, because it occurs whenever there is a mismatch between the reentrant visual representation and the ongoing lower level activity. Iterative reentrant processing was formalized in a computational model that provides an excellent fit to the data. The model provides a more comprehensive account of all forms of visual masking than do the long-held feed-forward views based on inhibitory contour interactions.


Subject(s)
Consciousness , Models, Psychological , Perceptual Masking/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Mental Processes , Neurons , Visual Perception
4.
Vision Res ; 38(15-16): 2489-505, 1998 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9798011

ABSTRACT

We show that early vision can use monocular cues to rapidly complete partially-occluded objects. Visual search for easily-detected fragments becomes difficult when the completed shape is similar to others in the display; conversely, search for fragments that are difficult to detect becomes easy when the completed shape is distinctive. Results indicate that completion occurs via the occlusion-triggered removal of occlusion edges and linking of associated regions. We fail to find evidence for a visible filling-in of contours or surfaces, but do find evidence for a 'functional' filling-in that prevents the constituent fragments from being rapidly accessed. As such, it is only the completed structures--and not the fragments themselves--that serve as the basis for rapid recognition.


Subject(s)
Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Cues , Humans , Psychophysics , Reaction Time , Visual Fields
5.
Psychol Rev ; 102(1): 101-30, 1995 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7878160

ABSTRACT

Experiments are presented showing that visual search for Mueller-Lyer stimuli is based on complete configurations rather than component segments. Segments easily detected in isolation were difficult to detect when embedded in a configuration, indicating preemption by low-level groups. This preemption--which caused stimulus components to become inaccessible to rapid search--was an all-or-nothing effect and so could serve as a powerful test of grouping. It is shown that these effects are unlikely to be due to blurring by simple spatial filters at early visual levels. It is proposed instead that they are due to more sophisticated processes that rapidly blind contour fragments into spatially extended assemblies. These results support the view that rapid visual search cannot access the primitive elements formed at the earliest stages of visual processing; rather, it can access only higher level, more ecologically relevant structures.


Subject(s)
Attention , Discrimination Learning , Field Dependence-Independence , Optical Illusions , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Humans , Orientation , Reaction Time , Size Perception
6.
Psychol Rev ; 98(3): 335-51, 1991 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1891522

ABSTRACT

It has generally been assumed that rapid visual search is based on simple features and that spatial relations between features are irrelevant for this task. Seven experiments involving search for line drawings contradict this assumption; a major determinant of search is the presence of line junctions. Arrow- and Y-junctions were detected rapidly in isolation and when they were embedded in drawings of rectangular polyhedra. Search for T-junctions was considerably slower. Drawings containing T-junctions often gave rise to very slow search even when distinguishing arrow- or Y-junctions were present. This sensitivity to line relations suggests that preattentive processes can extract 3-dimensional orientation from line drawings. A computational model is outlined for how this may be accomplished in early human vision.


Subject(s)
Attention , Depth Perception , Optical Illusions , Orientation , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Discrimination Learning , Humans , Reaction Time
7.
Science ; 247(4943): 721-3, 1990 Feb 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2300824

ABSTRACT

The task of visual search is to determine as rapidly as possible whether a target item is present or absent in a display. Rapidly detected items are thought to contain features that correspond to primitive elements in the human visual system. In previous theories, it has been assumed that visual search is based on simple two-dimensional features in the image. However, visual search also has access to another level of representation, one that describes properties in the corresponding three-dimensional scene. Among these properties are three dimensionality and the direction of lighting, but not viewing direction. These findings imply that the parallel processes of early vision are much more sophisticated than previously assumed.


Subject(s)
Visual Perception/physiology , Computers , Humans , Lighting , Space Perception
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...