Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 8 de 8
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Psychol Med ; 47(7): 1204-1214, 2017 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28052778

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) is often co-morbid with depression. Using the methods of network analysis, we computed two networks that disclose the potentially causal relationships among symptoms of these two disorders in 408 adult patients with primary OCD and co-morbid depression symptoms. METHOD: We examined the relationship between the symptoms constituting these syndromes by computing a (regularized) partial correlation network via the graphical LASSO procedure, and a directed acyclic graph (DAG) via a Bayesian hill-climbing algorithm. RESULTS: The results suggest that the degree of interference and distress associated with obsessions, and the degree of interference associated with compulsions, are the chief drivers of co-morbidity. Moreover, activation of the depression cluster appears to occur solely through distress associated with obsessions activating sadness - a key symptom that 'bridges' the two syndromic clusters in the DAG. CONCLUSIONS: Bayesian analysis can expand the repertoire of network analytic approaches to psychopathology. We discuss clinical implications and limitations of our findings.


Subject(s)
Bayes Theorem , Depression/physiopathology , Depressive Disorder/physiopathology , Models, Statistical , Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder/physiopathology , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Comorbidity , Depression/epidemiology , Depressive Disorder/epidemiology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder/epidemiology , Young Adult
2.
Behav Res Ther ; 34(11-12): 945-8, 1996.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8990547

ABSTRACT

Anxious individuals are slower at color-naming threat-related than nonthreat-related words in the emotional Stroop task. Recently, Mathews and Sebastian (1993, Cognition and Emotion, 7, 527-530) reported that this Stroop interference effect disappears when snake-fearful students are exposed to a snake while performing the color-naming task. In the present experiment, we had patients with social phobia and normal control subjects perform an emotional Stroop task under either low anxiety (i.e. upon entering the laboratory) or high anxiety (i.e. before giving a speech). Results indicated that Stroop interference for socially threatening words in the phobic group was suppressed under high anxiety. These findings may indicate that increased effort enables the subjects to suppress the interference produced in the Stroop task.


Subject(s)
Anxiety/psychology , Attention , Color Perception , Discrimination Learning , Phobic Disorders/psychology , Reading , Semantics , Adult , Anxiety/diagnosis , Arousal , Defense Mechanisms , Emotions , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Phobic Disorders/diagnosis , Reaction Time , Students/psychology
3.
Behav Res Ther ; 34(2): 157-62, 1996 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8741723

ABSTRACT

We employed Jacoby's white noise paradigm to investigate implicit memory bias for threat in panic disorder and in normal control subjects. Subjects heard a series of neutral sentences (e.g. "The shiny apple sat on the table") and panic sentences (e.g. "The anxious woman panicked in the supermarket"). Implicit memory for this prior exposure was then tested by having subjects rate the volume of white noise accompanying the presentation of 'old' sentences intermixed with 'new' sentences. Implicit memory for old sentences is revealed when subjects rate the white noise accompanying these sentences as less loud than noise accompanying new sentences. Results revealed that under low noise level, panic patients demonstrated an implicit memory bias for threat information, whereas control subjects did not. This differential priming effect suggests that information about threat may be automatically accessed in these patients.


Subject(s)
Memory , Panic Disorder/psychology , Adult , Aged , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Panic Disorder/diagnosis , Psychiatric Status Rating Scales
4.
Behav Res Ther ; 32(1): 119-22, 1994 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8135709

ABSTRACT

Panic disorder patients, obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) patients, and normal control subjects performed a computerized Stroop color-naming task in which they named the colors of panic-threat words (e.g. Collapse), general-threat words (e.g. Infectious), positive words related to panic (e.g. Relaxed), and neutral words (e.g. Sleepy). Idiographic stimulus selection ensured their personal emotional relevance for each subject. In accordance with prediction, panic patients, but not OCD patients, exhibited greater interference for panic-threat words than for positive words related to panic and for neutral words. Panic patients did not respond differentially to panic-threat and general-threat words. Complexities concerning attentional bias research in the anxiety disorders are discussed.


Subject(s)
Cognition , Emotions , Language , Panic Disorder/diagnosis , Female , Humans , Male , Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder/diagnosis , Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder/psychology , Panic Disorder/psychology , Psychiatric Status Rating Scales , Vocabulary
5.
Int J Eat Disord ; 14(4): 501-4, 1993 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8293033

ABSTRACT

Anorexia nervosa (AN) rarely develops after the age of 30 years, and rarely occurs in men. We report a case of chronic AN in a 72-year-old man, who reported a 20-year history of extreme low body weight, persistent fears of obesity, and feelings of being fat even at 93 lb. Also reported were episodes of self-induced vomiting, laxative abuse, and excessive exercising. Patient's scores on a battery of questionnaires were also consistent with a diagnosis of AN.


Subject(s)
Anorexia Nervosa/diagnosis , Aged , Anorexia Nervosa/psychology , Body Image , Body Weight , Diet, Reducing/psychology , Exercise/psychology , Humans , Male , Patient Care Team
6.
Behav Res Ther ; 30(2): 143-9, 1992 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1567343

ABSTRACT

Panic-disordered (PD) patients, obsessive-compulsive (OCD) patients, and normal control subjects were exposed to either a high (i.e. exercise) or low arousal manipulation prior to performing a computerized version of the modified Stroop color-naming paradigm. Subjects named the colors of neutral nonlexical stimuli, positive words, and threat words associated with fear, bodily sensations, and catastrophes. After the Stroop task, subjects rated the personal emotional significance of the words. Inconsistent with the emotionality hypothesis of Stroop interference, PD patients rated positive words as more emotional than catastrophe words, but took longer to color-name the latter than the former. Yet consistent with the emotionality hypothesis, PD patients took as long to color-name positive words as to color-name fear and bodily sensation words. Contrary to expectation, OCD patients resembled PD patients in terms of interference, and arousal did not enhance interference for threat words in PD patients.


Subject(s)
Arousal , Attention , Fear , Mental Processes , Panic Disorder/psychology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder/psychology , Semantics
7.
J Abnorm Psychol ; 99(4): 398-402, 1990 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2266215

ABSTRACT

Vietnam combat veterans with (n = 15) and without (n = 15) posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) performed a modified Stroop task in which they named the colors of neutral words (e.g., INPUT), positive words (e.g., LOVE), obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) words (e.g., GERMS), and PTSD words (e.g., BODYBAGS). In contrast to normal controls, PTSD patients took significantly longer to color-name PTSD words than to color-name neutral, OCD, and positive words. Because Stroop interference reflects involuntary semantic activation, it may provide a quantitative measure of intrusive cognitive activity--the hallmark symptom of PTSD.


Subject(s)
Arousal , Attention , Combat Disorders/psychology , Veterans/psychology , Adult , Color Perception , Humans , Male , Personality Tests , Semantics , Vietnam
8.
Behav Res Ther ; 28(5): 407-12, 1990.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2256899

ABSTRACT

A computerized Stroop color-naming paradigm was used to investigate attentional biases for selectively processing threat information in panic-disordered patients. Subjects named the colors of neutral words (e.g. TYPICAL), fear words (e.g. PANIC), bodily sensation words (e.g. HEARTBEAT), and catastrophe words (e.g. HEART ATTACK). To control for familiarity with threat concepts, we used clinicians who treat panic disorder as normal control subjects. In contrast to normal controls, panic patients exhibited greater Stroop interference for all threat words, especially those associated with catastrophe. Stroop interference waned during the course of the experiment, thereby indicating habituation to the semantic content of the cues. These findings suggest that panic disorder, like other anxiety disorders, is associated with an attentional bias for processing threatening information.


Subject(s)
Anxiety Disorders/psychology , Arousal , Attention , Cues , Panic , Adult , Color Perception , Female , Humans , Male , Semantics
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...