Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 5 de 5
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Cogn Res Princ Implic ; 7(1): 81, 2022 09 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36063244

ABSTRACT

Face coverings have been key in reducing the spread of COVID-19. At the same time, they have hindered interpersonal communication, particularly for those who rely on speechreading to aid communication. The available research indicated that deaf/hard of hearing (HoH) people experienced great difficulty communicating with people wearing masks and negative effects on wellbeing. Here we extended these findings by exploring which factors predict deaf/HoH people's communication difficulties, loss of information, and wellbeing. We also explored the factors predicting perceived usefulness of transparent face coverings and alternative ways of communicating. We report the findings from an accessible survey study, released in two written and three signed languages. Responses from 395 deaf/HoH UK and Spanish residents were collected online at a time when masks were mandatory. We investigated whether onset and level of deafness, knowledge of sign language, speechreading fluency, and country of residence predicted communication difficulties, wellbeing, and degree to which transparent face coverings were considered useful. Overall, deaf/HoH people and their relatives used masks most of the time despite greater communication difficulties. Late-onset deaf people were the group that experienced more difficulties in communication, and also reported lower wellbeing. However, both early- and late-onset deaf people reported missing more information and feeling more disconnected from society than HoH people. Finally, signers valued transparent face shields more positively than non-signers. The latter suggests that, while seeing the lips is positive to everyone, signers appreciate seeing the whole facial expression. Importantly, our data also revealed the importance of visual communication other than speechreading to facilitate face-to-face interactions.


Subject(s)
COVID-19 , Deafness , COVID-19/prevention & control , Communication , Humans , Masks , Sign Language
2.
Int J Lang Commun Disord ; 49(3): 343-53, 2014.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24617640

ABSTRACT

This study details the first ever investigation of narrative skills in a group of 17 deaf signing children who have been diagnosed with disorders in their British Sign Language development compared with a control group of 17 deaf child signers matched for age, gender, education, quantity, and quality of language exposure and non-verbal intelligence. Children were asked to generate a narrative based on events in a language free video. Narratives were analysed for global structure, information content and local level grammatical devices, especially verb morphology. The language-impaired group produced shorter, less structured and grammatically simpler narratives than controls, with verb morphology particularly impaired. Despite major differences in how sign and spoken languages are articulated, narrative is shown to be a reliable marker of language impairment across the modality boundaries.


Subject(s)
Aptitude , Deafness/rehabilitation , Language Development Disorders/rehabilitation , Narration , Sign Language , Adolescent , Child , Child, Preschool , Education of Hearing Disabled , Female , Humans , Language Development Disorders/diagnosis , Male , Psycholinguistics , Reference Values , Semantics , Speech Production Measurement
3.
J Psycholinguist Res ; 43(5): 587-610, 2014 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24043510

ABSTRACT

We used fluency tasks to investigate lexical organisation in Deaf adults who use British sign language (BSL). The number of responses produced to semantic categories did not differ from reports in spoken languages. However, there was considerable variability in the number of responses across phonological categories, and some signers had difficulty retrieving items. Responses were richly clustered according to semantic and/or phonological properties. With respect to phonology, there was significantly more clustering around the parameters "handshape" and "location" compared to "movement". We conclude that the BSL lexicon is organised in similar ways to the lexicons of spoken languages, but that lexical retrieval is characterised by strong links between semantics and phonology; movement is less readily retrieved than handshape and location; and phonological fluency is difficult for signers because they have little metaphonological awareness in BSL and because signs do not display the onset salience that characterises spoken words.


Subject(s)
Deafness/psychology , Linguistics , Sign Language , Adult , Humans , Middle Aged , Young Adult
4.
J Child Lang ; 40(1): 193-220, 2013 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22717181

ABSTRACT

We adapted the semantic fluency task into British Sign Language (BSL). In Study 1, we present data from twenty-two deaf signers aged four to fifteen. We show that the same 'cognitive signatures' that characterize this task in spoken languages are also present in deaf children, for example, the semantic clustering of responses. In Study 2, we present data from thirteen deaf children with Specific Language Impairment (SLI) in BSL, in comparison to a subset of children from Study 1 matched for age and BSL exposure. The two groups' results were comparable in most respects. However, the group with SLI made occasional word-finding errors and gave fewer responses in the first 15 seconds. We conclude that deaf children with SLI do not differ from their controls in terms of the semantic organization of the BSL lexicon, but that they access signs less efficiently.


Subject(s)
Language Development Disorders/psychology , Persons With Hearing Impairments/psychology , Semantics , Sign Language , Adolescent , Case-Control Studies , Child , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Language Development Disorders/complications , Male
5.
Br J Dev Psychol ; 28(Pt 1): 33-49, 2010 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20306624

ABSTRACT

This paper presents the first ever group study of specific language impairment (SLI) in users of sign language. A group of 50 children were referred to the study by teachers and speech and language therapists. Individuals who fitted pre-determined criteria for SLI were then systematically assessed. Here, we describe in detail the performance of 13 signing deaf children aged 5-14 years on normed tests of British Sign Language (BSL) sentence comprehension, repetition of nonsense signs, expressive grammar and narrative skills, alongside tests of non-verbal intelligence and fine motor control. Results show these children to have a significant language delay compared to their peers matched for age and language experience. This impaired development cannot be explained by poor exposure to BSL, or by lower general cognitive, social, or motor abilities. As is the case for SLI in spoken languages, we find heterogeneity within the group in terms of which aspects of language are affected and the severity of the impairment. We discuss the implications of the existence of language impairments in a sign language for theories of SLI and clinical practice.


Subject(s)
Deafness/complications , Deafness/rehabilitation , Language Development Disorders/diagnosis , Sign Language , Adolescent , Child , Child, Preschool , Comprehension , Female , Humans , Intelligence , Language Development Disorders/complications , Language Tests/statistics & numerical data , Linguistics/statistics & numerical data , Male , Motor Skills , Nonverbal Communication , Severity of Illness Index , United Kingdom
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...