Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 17 de 17
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Psychol Sport Exerc ; 70: 102537, 2024 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37703938

ABSTRACT

We examined the naturalness bias - the tendency to prefer people who seem to come by their talent naturally and not through work or effort - on ratings of athletic ability. Football (soccer) coaches, athletes, fans, and non-fans (n = 430) read about an athlete described as either being a natural or a striver. After watching a video of the athlete going through drills, participants rated the athlete on likelihood of success, athletic skill, mental strength, and physiological ability. Participants of all expertise levels tended to rate the athlete described as a natural as being higher in mental toughness than did the participants rating the athlete described as being a striver. There were no significant differences in ratings for the natural and the striver for measures of skill and explosiveness. It appears that the naturalness bias was most likely to influence ratings not easily judged by viewing a single performance.


Subject(s)
Soccer , Humans , Soccer/physiology , Athletes
2.
Front Psychol ; 10: 1452, 2019.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31333529

ABSTRACT

The present study sought to determine whether witness memory for duration could be improved. In three studies, we examined the effects of unpacking (breaking an event into its component parts), anchoring (supplying participants with a reference duration), and summation (summing component estimates). Participants watched a video-recorded mock crime and provided duration estimates for components of the crime (e.g., casing the car, unlocking the door, etc.) and for the total crime. Results indicate that bias in estimated duration was less for the sum of the parts than it was for the overall duration estimate. Further, the sum of the individual parts did not equal the total estimate, even though all estimates were given in sequence. Summing the component parts could be a more successful intervention than anchoring or unpacking and is easy to employ with witnesses.

3.
Acta Psychol (Amst) ; 192: 153-162, 2019 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30529827

ABSTRACT

We examined whether or not interventions that have been used to try to influence predictions of future task duration - unpacking, summing and anchoring - had a similar effect on retrospective estimations of duration. In three studies, participants experienced a number of short stimuli, such as watching videos, before estimating the duration for each of the stimuli and the overall duration. The first estimation given served as an anchor for all following estimates. If the first estimation was highly biased in one direction, then subsequent estimates were more likely to also be biased in the same direction. Additionally, separate estimates for a number of individual tasks differed from the estimates for all of the tasks combined. This incongruity happened even though all estimates were given in sequence. Overall, results indicated that memories of past task duration could be influenced by the manner in which they were elicited.


Subject(s)
Memory/physiology , Time Perception/physiology , Adult , Female , Forecasting , Humans , Male
4.
Front Psychol ; 6: 2016, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26779110

ABSTRACT

People high in rumination are good at tasks that require persistence whereas people low in rumination is good at tasks that require flexibility. Here we examine real world implications of these differences in dynamic, team sport. In two studies, we found that professional male football (soccer) players from Germany and female field hockey players on the US national team were lower in rumination than were non-athletes. Further, low levels of rumination were associated with a longer career at a higher level in football players. Results indicate that athletes in dynamic, team sport might benefit from the flexibility associated with being low in rumination.

5.
Front Psychol ; 5: 624, 2014.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24994995
6.
J Appl Soc Psychol ; 43(8)2013 Aug 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24273339

ABSTRACT

We examined whether people recognized that others might disagree with their high self-assessments of driving ability, and, if so, why. Participants in four experiments expressed a belief that others would assess them as worse drivers than they assessed themselves. This difference appears to be caused by participants' use of their own, idiosyncratic definition of driving ability. In Experiments 2 and 3 participants reported that others would supply similar assessments of their ability when the skill was less ambiguous. Results of Experiment 4 indicate that participants recognize that there may be more than one way to view driving performance. Participants appear aware that others likely disagree with their self-assessment of driving ability due to differences in how others define driving ability.

7.
Organ Behav Hum Decis Process ; 122(2): 200-213, 2013 Nov 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24159253

ABSTRACT

People have been shown to view their beliefs as being prototypical (modal) but their abilities as (falsely) unique (above or below average). It is possible that these two viewpoints - self as prototypical and self as unique - can be reconciled. If the distribution of ability for a given skill is skewed such that many others have high (low) ability and few others have low (high) ability, it is possible that a majority of peoples' self-assessments can be above (below) average. Participants in 5 studies demonstrated an understanding that various skills have skewed ability distributions and their self-assessments were related to distribution shape: high when negatively skewed and low when positively skewed. Further, participants tended to place themselves near the mode of their perceived skill distribution. Participants were most likely to think that they were good at skills for which they thought that most others were also good.

8.
Psychol Health ; 29(1): 64-80, 2013 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23947805

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: Although traditional models posit that negative emotional responses to stress increase cardiovascular reactivity (CVR), laboratory studies have generally not found a strong emotion-CVR association. In this paper, we took a multidimensional approach to examining psychological reactions to stress in three studies. METHODS: In each study we assessed the amount of effort exerted by a participant and the negative affect (NA) felt by the participant with different self-reported measures and an effort behavioural measure. RESULTS: Our findings consistently demonstrated that NA was associated with CVR when effort was relatively high, but not when effort was relatively low. CONCLUSION: This suggests that the weak NA-CVR correlations reported in past research may have been confounded by a third effort-related variable and that CVR is significantly associated with NA under certain circumstances. Furthermore, our findings suggest that by considering the multidimensional nature of psychological responses to stress, we may come to better understand the links between stress-related emotion and physiology.


Subject(s)
Affect/physiology , Blood Pressure/physiology , Heart Rate/physiology , Physical Exertion/physiology , Stress, Psychological/physiopathology , Female , Humans , Speech/physiology , Surveys and Questionnaires
9.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 66(1): 121-37, 2013.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22928697

ABSTRACT

People are often wrong in estimating both how long tasks have taken in the past and how long they will take in the future. Bias could be due to factors such as task involvement, an individual's engagement or motivation in completing the task, or aspects of the task such as its relative duration or memory storage size associated with it. We examined time estimation bias in actors (likely to experience high levels of task involvement) and observers (likely to experience low levels of task involvement) for both predictions of and memory of task duration. Results suggest that bias appears to be due to memory storage size rather than to involvement with the task.


Subject(s)
Memory/physiology , Motivation/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Time Perception/physiology , Bias , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation , Predictive Value of Tests , Reaction Time , Time Factors
10.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 18(5): 827-32, 2011 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21861201

ABSTRACT

Three studies confirm the existence of the return trip effect: The return trip often seems shorter than the initial trip, even though the distance traveled and the actual time spent traveling are identical. A pretest shows that people indeed experience a return trip effect regularly, and the effect was found on a bus trip (Study 1), a bicycle trip (Study 2), and when participants watched a video of someone else traveling (Study 3). The return trip effect also existed when another, equidistant route was taken on the return trip, showing that it is not familiarity with the route that causes this effect. Rather, it seems that a violation of expectations causes this effect.


Subject(s)
Time Perception , Travel/psychology , Anticipation, Psychological , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation , Recognition, Psychology , Time Factors , Video Recording , Visual Perception , Young Adult
11.
J Exp Psychol Appl ; 14(3): 266-75, 2008 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18808280

ABSTRACT

People are often inaccurate in predicting task duration. The memory bias explanation holds that this error is due to people having incorrect memories of how long previous tasks have taken, and these biased memories cause biased predictions. Therefore, the authors examined the effect on increasing predictive accuracy of correcting memory through supplying feedback for actual task duration. For Experiments 1 (paper-counting task) and 2 (essay-writing task), college students were supplied with duration information about their previous performance on a similar task before predicting task duration. For Experiment 3, participants were recruited at various locations, such as fast food restaurants and video arcades, and supplied with average task duration for others before predicting how long the task would take. In all 3 experiments, supplying feedback increased predictive accuracy. Overall, results indicate that, when predicting duration, people do well when they rely not on memory of past task duration but instead on measures of actual duration, whether their own or that of others.


Subject(s)
Culture , Feedback, Psychological , Mental Recall , Time Perception , Adolescent , Adult , Discrimination Learning , Female , Humans , Male
12.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 15(1): 202-7, 2008 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18605504

ABSTRACT

Vierordt's (1868) law states that when estimating the duration of a previous task, people overestimate short durations and underestimate long ones. We examine whether this same pattern holds for remembered and predicted duration for tasks lasting between 1 and 15 min. In support of Vierordt's law and its extension to future duration estimates, task duration tended to be overestimated for short tasks (less than 2 min) and underestimated for long tasks for both remembered and predicted duration.


Subject(s)
Attention , Culture , Mental Recall , Time Perception , Discrimination Learning , Humans , Perceptual Distortion , Psychophysics
13.
Mem Cognit ; 35(3): 557-64, 2007 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17691153

ABSTRACT

Both anecdotal accounts and experimental evidence suggest that people underestimate how long it will take them to complete future tasks. A possible reason for this tendency is that people remember tasks as taking less time than they actually did, with these biased memories causing a corresponding bias in prediction. Two experiments were performed to determine whether or not a systematic bias in memory could explain a similar systematic bias in prediction. In support, it was found that (1) the tendency to underestimate future duration disappears when the task is novel, (2) there is similar bias in estimation of both past and future durations, and (3) variables that affect memory of duration, such as level of experience with the task and duration of delay before estimation, affect prediction of duration in the same way. It appears that, at least in part, people underestimate future event duration because they underestimate past event duration.


Subject(s)
Memory , Psychology/methods , Time Perception , Bias , Cognition , Female , Forecasting , Humans , Male , Predictive Value of Tests , Psychology/statistics & numerical data
14.
Psychol Sci ; 16(9): 743-4, 2005 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16137262
15.
Psychol Bull ; 131(5): 738-56, 2005 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16187856

ABSTRACT

People frequently underestimate how long it will take them to complete a task. The prevailing view is that during the prediction process, people incorrectly use their memories of how long similar tasks have taken in the past because they take an overly optimistic outlook. A variety of evidence is reviewed in this article that points to a different, although not mutually exclusive, explanation: People base predictions of future duration on their memories of how long past events have taken, but these memories are systematic underestimates of past duration. People appear to underestimate future event duration because they underestimate past event duration.


Subject(s)
Memory , Time Perception , Forecasting , Humans , Time Factors
16.
Psychol Bull ; 131(5): 761-2, 2005 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16187858

ABSTRACT

People chronically underestimate how long tasks will take. In their original article, the present authors (M. M. Roy, N. J. S. Christenfeld, & C. R. M. McKenzie, suggested a simple, broadly applicable explanation: Biased predictions result from biased memories. In their comment article, D. Griffin and R. Buehler suggested that in many domains in which this memory-bias account appears to out-predict their own account, theirs actually makes no prediction at all. However, the present authors did not suggest that only 1 theory is right but that theirs is consistent with data that prior theories, including their own, cannot explain. Ignoring memories of past tasks is not a complete explanation for the phenomenon if the memories people could consult are themselves biased. Nonetheless, underestimating future task duration is almost certainly multiply determined, and thus our account and theirs can coexist.


Subject(s)
Attitude , Memory , Humans
17.
Psychol Sci ; 15(5): 361-3, 2004 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15102149

ABSTRACT

We examined whether the frequent casual reports of people resembling their pets are accurate by having observers attempt to match dogs with their owners. We further explored whether any ability of observers to make such matches is due to people selecting dogs who resemble them, in which case the resemblance should be greater for predictable purebreds than for nonpurebreds, or is due to convergence, in which case the resemblance should grow with duration of ownership. Forty-five dogs and their owners were photographed separately, and judges were shown one owner, that owner's dog, and one other dog, with the task of picking out the true match. The results were consistent with a selection account: Observers were able to match only purebred dogs with their owners, and there was no relation between the ability to pair a person with his or her pet and the time they had cohabited. The ability to match people and pets did not seem to rely on any simple trait matching (e.g., size or hairiness). The results suggest that when people pick a pet, they seek one that, at some level, resembles them, and when they get a purebred, they get what they want.


Subject(s)
Dogs , Face , Ownership , Adult , Animals , Female , Humans , Judgment , Male , Random Allocation
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...