Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 13 de 13
Filter
1.
Comput Biol Chem ; 80: 66-78, 2019 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30928870

ABSTRACT

In this study, the structural and antioxidant behavior of the three lichen-derived natural compounds such as atranorin (AT), evernic acid (EV) and diffractaic acid (DF) has been investigated in the gas and water phase using both B3LYP and M06-2X functional level of density functional theory (DFT) with two different basis sets 6-31+G (d, p) and 6-311++G (d, p). The intramolecular H-bonds (IHB) strength, aromaticity and noncovalent interactions (NCI) have been computed with the help of the quantum theory of atoms in molecules (QTAIM). This calculation gives major structural characteristics that indirectly influence the antioxidant behavior of the investigated compounds. The spin density (SD) delocalization of the unpaired electron is found to be the main stabilizing factor of neutral and cationic radical species. The main mechanisms, recommended in the literature, for the antioxidant action of polyphenols as radical scavengers such as hydrogen atom transfer (HAT), single electron transfer followed by proton transfer (SET-PT), and sequential proton loss electron transfer (SPLET), were examined. The result shows that the HAT and SPLET mechanism are the most conceivable one for the antioxidant action of this class of compounds in gas and water phase respectively. Preference of SPLET over HAT in water phase is due to the significantly lower value of proton affinity (PA) compared to the bond dissociation enthalpy (BDE) value. This study reveals that O2-H3, O9-H26 and O4-H45 respectively are the most favored site of AT, EV and DF for homolytic as well as heterolytic OH bond breaking.


Subject(s)
Anisoles/chemistry , Antioxidants/chemistry , Hydroxybenzoates/chemistry , Density Functional Theory , Hydrogen Bonding , Models, Chemical , Molecular Conformation , Oxidation-Reduction , Protons , Thermodynamics , Water/chemistry
2.
Comput Biol Chem ; 77: 154-166, 2018 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30321850

ABSTRACT

Developments of novel inhibitors to prevent the function of 5-lipoxygenase (5-LOX) proteins that are responsible for a variety of inflammatory and allergic disease are a major challenge in the scientific community. In this study, robust QSAR classification models for predicting 5-LOX activity were developed using machine learning algorithms. The Support Vector Machines (SVM), Logistic Regression, k-Nearest Neighbour (NN) and Decision Trees were adopted to improve the prediction ability of the classification models. The most informative molecular descriptors that contribute to the prediction of 5-LOX activity are screened from e-Dragon, Ochem, PowerMV and Combined databases using Filter-based feature selection methods such as Correlation Feature Selection (CFS) and Information Gain (IG). Performances of the models were measured by 5-fold cross-validation and external test sets prediction. Evaluation of performance of feature selection revealed that the CFS method outperforms the IG method for all descriptor databases except for PowerMV database. The best ensemble classification model was obtained with the IG filtered 'PowerMV' descriptor database using kNN (k = 5) algorithm which displayed an overall accuracy of 76.6% for the training set and 77.9% for the test set. Finally, we employed this model as a virtual screening tool for identifying potential 5-LOX inhibitors from the e-Drug3D drug database and found 43 potential hit candidates. This top screened hits containing one known 5-LOX inhibitors zileuton as well as novel scaffolds. These compounds further screened by applying molecular docking simulation and identified four potential hits such as Belinostat, Masoprocol, Mefloquine and Sitagliptin having a comparable binding affinity to zileuton.


Subject(s)
Arachidonate 5-Lipoxygenase/metabolism , Drug Evaluation, Preclinical , Lipoxygenase Inhibitors/analysis , Lipoxygenase Inhibitors/chemistry , Molecular Docking Simulation , Quantitative Structure-Activity Relationship , Humans , Lipoxygenase Inhibitors/pharmacology , Logistic Models
3.
Acta Psychol (Amst) ; 137(2): 226-34, 2011 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20979989

ABSTRACT

Recently, it has been shown that patients with visual extinction can show enhanced awareness of contralesional stimuli that match the contents of working memory [Soto, D., & Humphreys, G.W. (2006). Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 103, 4789-4792]. Here we investigate whether these effects extend to cases where the working memory cue is verbal rather than visual (Study 1), and we show that non-spatial cues can overcome problems in spatial disengagement in patients, and this can affect the first eye movement made in search (Study 2). We discuss the implications of the data for understanding the relations between the cueing of attention from visual and conceptual information held in working memory and the cueing of spatial attention, and for understanding the relations between non-spatial and spatial biases in selection.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Eye Movements/physiology , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Aged , Brain/physiopathology , Cues , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Photic Stimulation , Reaction Time/physiology , Stroke/physiopathology
4.
J Neurol ; 257(11): 1812-21, 2010 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20556413

ABSTRACT

A total of 29 patients with homonymous visual field defects without neglect practised visual search in 20 daily sessions, over a period of 4 weeks. Patients searched for a single randomly positioned target amongst distractors displayed for 3 s. After training patients demonstrated significantly shorter reaction times for search stimuli (Pambakian et al. in J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 75:1443-1448, 2004). In this study, patients achieved improved search efficiency after training by altering their oculomotor behaviour in the following ways: (1) patients directed a higher proportion of fixations into the hemispace containing the target, (2) patients were quicker to saccade into the hemifield containing the target if the initial saccade had been made into the opposite hemifield, (3) patients made fewer transitions from one hemifield to another before locating the target, (4) patients made a larger initial saccade, although the direction of the initial saccade did not change as a result of training, (5) patients acquired a larger visual lobe in their blind hemifield after training. Patients also required fewer saccades to locate the target after training reflecting improved search efficiency. All these changes were confined to the training period and maintained at follow-up. Taken together these results suggest that visual training facilitates the development of specific compensatory eye movement strategies in patients with homonymous visual field defects.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Physiological/physiology , Eye Movements/physiology , Hemianopsia/rehabilitation , Photic Stimulation/methods , Humans , Visual Fields
5.
Vision Res ; 50(16): 1590-7, 2010 Jul 21.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20493900

ABSTRACT

Oculomotor capture can occur automatically in a bottom-up way through the sudden appearance of a new object or in a top-down fashion when a stimulus in the array matches the contents of working memory. However, it is not clear whether or not working memory processing can influence the early stages of oculomotor capture by abrupt onsets. Here we present clear evidence for an early modulation driven by stimulus matches to the contents of working memory in the colour dimension. Interestingly, verbal as well as visual information in working memory influenced the direction of the fastest saccades made in search, saccadic latencies and the curvature of the scan paths made to the search target. This pattern of results arose even though the contents of working memory were detrimental for search, demonstrating an early, automatic top-down mediation of oculomotor onset capture by the contents of working memory.


Subject(s)
Attention , Eye Movements/physiology , Memory, Short-Term , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Female , Fixation, Ocular , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation/methods , Reaction Time , Young Adult
7.
Prog Brain Res ; 171: 559-62, 2008.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18718353

ABSTRACT

Previous studies have produced contradictory evidence on the nature of the visual search impairment in patients with Parkinson's disease (PD). Eye movements were measured during multi-target search in nine individuals with mild-to-moderate PD. Subjects were asked to click on a response button whenever they judged they were fixating a target for the first time. Compared to age-matched healthy volunteers, PD patients were impaired at efficient search (detecting "+"s amongst "L"s) but not inefficient search ("T"s amongst "L"s). However, these patients had normal memory for locations as indexed by their rate of re-clicking on previously inspected locations. We suggest that the pattern of gaze for efficient search may reflect impaired saliency processing in PD.


Subject(s)
Eye Movements/physiology , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Parkinson Disease/physiopathology , Visual Perception/physiology , Aged , Humans , Memory/physiology , Middle Aged
8.
Singapore Med J ; 49(2): e33-4, 2008 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18301822

ABSTRACT

Pleomorphic adenoma, or chondroid syringoma (CS), is a rare, benign, skin appendageal tumour. Because of the unremarkable clinical presentation of this rare tumour, the diagnosis is made on microscopic examination. The usual presentation is of an asymptomatic, slowly-growing mass, typically located in the head and neck region. We present a case of a CS located over the forehead of a 32-year-old man. In the evaluation of a middle-aged male patient with a small cutaneous nodule in the head and neck region, chondroid syringoma should also be considered in the differential diagnosis. For such a lesion, excisional biopsy, without destroying the aesthetic and functional structures, is the preferred diagnostic, as well as therapeutic approach.


Subject(s)
Adenoma, Pleomorphic/pathology , Forehead/pathology , Sweat Gland Neoplasms/pathology , Adenoma, Pleomorphic/diagnosis , Adult , Humans , Male , Sweat Gland Neoplasms/diagnosis
9.
Exp Psychol ; 55(5): 301-12, 2008.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25116297

ABSTRACT

Searching a cluttered visual scene for a specific item of interest can take several seconds to perform if the target item is difficult to discriminate from surrounding items. Whether working memory processes are utilized to guide the path of attentional selection during such searches remains under debate. Previous studies have found evidence to support a role for spatial working memory in inefficient search, but the role of nonspatial working memory remains unclear. Here, we directly compared the role of spatial and nonspatial working memory for both an efficient and inefficient search task. In Experiment 1, we used a dual-task paradigm to investigate the effect of performing visual search within the retention interval of a spatial working memory task. Importantly, by incorporating two working memory loads (low and high) we were able to make comparisons between dual-task conditions, rather than between dual-task and single-task conditions. This design allows any interference effects observed to be attributed to changes in memory load, rather than to nonspecific effects related to "dual-task" performance. We found that the efficiency of the inefficient search task declined as spatial memory load increased, but that the efficient search task remained efficient. These results suggest that spatial memory plays an important role in inefficient but not efficient search. In Experiment 2, participants performed the same visual search tasks within the retention interval of visually matched spatial and verbal working memory tasks. Critically, we found comparable dual-task interference between inefficient search and both the spatial and nonspatial working memory tasks, indicating that inefficient search recruits working memory processes common to both domains.


Subject(s)
Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Eye Movement Measurements , Female , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Humans , Male , Perceptual Masking/physiology , Pilot Projects , Psychological Tests , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Reaction Time , Young Adult
10.
Neuropsychologia ; 44(5): 731-43, 2006.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16150470

ABSTRACT

The functional-neuroanatomic relationship that describes the involvement of the parietal cortex in visual search was investigated using repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS; 10 Hz, 500 ms in duration). Twelve adult participants performed feature-based visual search for a unique letter-without eye movements-under conditions that involved manipulations of search efficiency (efficient versus inefficient) and target-selection demands (set-size: 4 versus 10). rTMS was applied over the right posterior parietal cortex at the onset of the search array for all factorial conditions (0-500 ms); stimulation was additionally administered at 500 ms post-array onset (500-1000 ms) during inefficient search (set-size 10). Stimulation over the primary sensorimotor cortex served as a within-subjects control condition, and eye movements were monitored continuously. Significant increases in reaction time were restricted to parietal stimulation during inefficient search (set-size 10), with interference observed when rTMS was administered at the onset of the search array and at 500 ms post-array onset. The early effect was confined to target-present trials and the late effect was confined to target-absent trials, which may indicate temporally dissociable parietal involvement in target detection and response-based selection and/or search termination, respectively. Error rates did not vary significantly as a function of any of the independent variables. Taken together, these results are consistent with evidence from functional magnetic resonance studies indicating that inefficient feature-based visual search requires an intact parietal cortex, and also indicate that the parietal cortex is involved in inefficient search later than has been previously reported.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Parietal Lobe/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Brain Mapping , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation/methods , Reaction Time , Time Factors , Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation/methods
11.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 17(2): 340-54, 2005 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15811244

ABSTRACT

Right-hemisphere patients with left neglect often demonstrate abnormal visual search, re-examining stimuli to the right while ignoring those to the left. But re-fixations alone do not reveal if patients misjudge whether they have searched a location before. Here, we not only tracked the eye movements of 16 neglect patients during search, but also asked them to click a response button only when they judged they were fixating a target for the very first time. ''Re-clicking'' on previously found targets would indicate that patients erroneously respond to these as new discoveries. Lesions were mapped with high-resolution MRI. Neglect patients with damage involving the right intraparietal sulcus or right inferior frontal lobe ''re-clicked'' on previously found targets on the right at a pathological rate, whereas those with medial occipito-temporal lesions did not. For the intraparietal sulcus patients, the probability of erroneous re-clicks on an old target increased with time since first discovering it; whereas for frontal patients it was independent of search time, suggesting different underlying mechanisms in these two types of patient. Re-click deficits correlated with degree of leftward neglect, mainly due to both being severe in intraparietal cases. These results demonstrate that misjudging previously searched locations for new ones can contribute to pathological search in neglect, with potentially different mechanisms being involved in intraparietal versus inferior frontal patients. When combined with a spatial bias to the right, such deficits might explain why many neglect patients often re-examine rightward locations, at the expense of items to their left.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Brain Injuries/physiopathology , Frontal Lobe/physiopathology , Parietal Lobe/physiopathology , Perceptual Disorders/physiopathology , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Brain Mapping , Eye Movements/physiology , Female , Frontal Lobe/pathology , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged , Parietal Lobe/pathology , Perceptual Masking/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Reaction Time/physiology
12.
Brain ; 126(Pt 9): 1986-97, 2003 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12821519

ABSTRACT

The brain regions that are critically associated with visual neglect have become intensely disputed. In particular, one study of middle cerebral artery (MCA) stroke patients has claimed that the key brain region associated with neglect is the mid portion of the superior temporal gyrus (STG), on the lateral surface of the right hemisphere, rather than the posterior parietal lobe. Such a result has wide-ranging implications for both our understanding of the normal function these cortical areas and the potential mechanisms underlying neglect. Here, we use novel high resolution MRI protocols to map the lesions of 35 right-hemisphere patients who had suffered either MCA or posterior cerebral artery (PCA) territory stroke. For patients with MCA territory strokes, the critical area involved in all neglect patients was the angular gyrus of the inferior parietal lobe (IPL). Although the STG was damaged in half of our MCA neglect patients, it was spared in the rest. For PCA territory strokes, all patients with neglect had lesions involving the parahippocampal region, on the medial surface of the temporal lobe. PCA patients without neglect did not have damage to this area. We conclude that damage to two posterior regions, one in the IPL and the other in the medial temporal lobe, is associated with neglect. Although some neglect patients do have damage to the STG, our findings challenge the recent influential proposal that lesions of this area are critically associated with neglect. Instead, our results implicate the angular gyrus and parahippocampal region in this role.


Subject(s)
Infarction, Middle Cerebral Artery/pathology , Infarction, Posterior Cerebral Artery/pathology , Perceptual Disorders/pathology , Visual Perception , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Brain Mapping/methods , Female , Humans , Infarction, Middle Cerebral Artery/psychology , Infarction, Posterior Cerebral Artery/psychology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Male , Middle Aged , Parahippocampal Gyrus/pathology , Parietal Lobe/pathology , Perceptual Disorders/etiology , Prospective Studies
13.
Neuroimage ; 18(2): 231-46, 2003 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12595178

ABSTRACT

A saccade involves both a step in eye position and an obligatory shift in spatial attention. The traditional division of saccades into two types, the "reflexive" saccade made in response to an exogenous stimulus change in the visual periphery and the "voluntary" saccade based on an endogenous judgement to move gaze, is supported by lines of evidence which include the longer onset latency of the latter and the differential effects of lesions in humans and primates on each. It has been supposed that differences between the two types of saccade derive from differences in how the spatial attention shifts involved in each are processed. However, while functional imaging studies have affirmed the close link between saccades and attentional shifts by showing they activate overlapping cortical networks, attempts to contrast exogenous with endogenous ("covert") attentional shifts directly have not revealed separate patterns of cortical activation. We took the "overt" approach, contrasting whole reflexive and voluntary saccades using event-related fMRI. This demonstrated that, relative to reflexive saccades, voluntary saccades produced greater activation within the frontal eye fields and the saccade-related area of the intraparietal sulci. The reverse contrast showed reflexive saccades to be associated with relative activation of the angular gyrus of the inferior parietal lobule, strongest in the right hemisphere. The frequent involvement of the right inferior parietal lobule in lesions causing hemispatial neglect has long implicated this parietal region in an important, though as yet uncertain, role in the awareness and exploration of space. This is the first study to demonstrate preferential activation of an area in its posterior part, the right angular gyrus, during production of exogenously triggered rather than endogenously generated saccades, a finding which we propose is consistent with an important role for the angular gyrus in exogenous saccadic orienting.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Imaging, Three-Dimensional , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Reflex/physiology , Saccades/physiology , Adult , Arousal/physiology , Brain Mapping , Dominance, Cerebral/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Nerve Net/physiology , Orientation/physiology , Oxygen Consumption/physiology , Parietal Lobe/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Visual Fields/physiology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...