Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 3 de 3
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
J Comp Physiol A ; 176(3): 347-54, 1995 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7707270

ABSTRACT

1. Flights of three big brown bats (Eptesicus fuscus) landing on a hand and catching a suspended mealworm were video analysed. 2. Results were consistent with the bats using the same basic control procedure in the quite different approach tasks--namely keeping tau (r) = kr and tau (a)/tau (r) = k alpha r. Here r is the current distance to the destination; alpha is the angle between the current direction of the destination and the goal direction of final approach (beta min); tau (r) = r/r, tau (alpha) = alpha/alpha; and kr, k alpha r are constants. 3. The bats were each quite consistent on a particular task (hand or mealworm) in the values they used for the control parameters kr, k alpha r and beta min. However, different values were used in the two tasks, which reflected the different behaviour required at the destination. Flights to hand required twisting and landing upside down and approach angle beta min was closer to vertical and kr was smaller and corresponded to decelerating nearly to a stop. In contrast, the mealworms were caught in mid flight and approach angle beta min was shallower and speed of approach was about constant. 4. tau (r) might be registered acoustically by tau (echo-delay) or by tau (echo-intensity). tau (alpha) might be registered by the bat's directional hearing and gravity sense. 5. The bat's learned the tasks easily, suggesting that the control procedure they used in the experiments was part and parcel of the natural skills they had developed in the wild.


Subject(s)
Chiroptera/physiology , Echolocation/physiology , Flight, Animal/physiology , Acoustics , Animals , Feeding Behavior/physiology
2.
J Exp Biol ; 194: 285-98, 1994 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7964403

ABSTRACT

Certain tiger moths emit high-frequency clicks to an attacking bat, causing it to break off its pursuit. The sounds may either orient the bat by providing it with information that it uses to make an attack decision (aposematism) or they may disorient the bat by interrupting the normal flow of echo information required to complete a successful capture (startle, jamming). At what point during a bat's attack does an arctiid emit its clicks? If the sounds are aposematic, the moth should emit them early in the attack echolocation sequence in order to allow the bat time to understand their meaning. If, however, the sounds disrupt the bat's echo-processing behaviour, one would expect them to be emitted later in the attack to maximize their confusion effects. To test this, we exposed dogbane tiger moths (Cycnia tenera) to a recording of the echolocation sequence emitted by a big brown bat (Eptesicus fuscus) as it attacked a stationary target. Our results demonstrate that, at normal echolocation intensities, C. tenera does not respond to approach calls but waits until the terminal phase of the attack before emitting its clicks. This timing is evident whether the moth is stationary or flying and is largely independent of the intensity of the echolocation calls. These results support the hypothesis of a jamming effect (e.g. 'phantom echoes') and suggest that, to determine experimentally the effects of arctiid clicks on bats, it is important that the bats be tested under conditions that simulate the natural context in which this defence operates.


Subject(s)
Chiroptera/physiology , Echolocation/physiology , Moths/physiology , Vocalization, Animal/physiology , Acoustics , Animals , Female , Male , Periodicity
3.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 94(5): 2691-712, 1993 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8270744

ABSTRACT

The spectrogram correlation and transformation (SCAT) model of the sonar receiver in the big brown bat (Eptesicus fuscus) consists of a cochlear component for encoding the bat's frequency modulated (FM) sonar transmissions and multiple FM echoes in a spectrogram format, followed by two parallel pathways for processing temporal and spectral information in sonar echoes to reconstruct the absolute range and fine range structure of multiple targets from echo spectrograms. The outputs of computations taking place along these parallel pathways converge to be displayed along a computed image dimension of echo delay or target range. The resulting image depicts the location of various reflecting sources in different targets along the range axis. This series of transforms is equivalent to simultaneous, parallel forward and inverse transforms on sonar echoes, yielding the impulse responses of targets by deconvolution of the spectrograms. The performance of the model accurately reproduces the images perceived by Eptesicus in a variety of behavioral experiments on two-glint resolution in range, echo phase sensitivity, amplitude-latency trading of range estimates, dissociation of time- and frequency-domain image components, and ranging accuracy in noise.


Subject(s)
Auditory Perception/physiology , Chiroptera , Echolocation/physiology , Mathematical Computing , Sound Localization/physiology , Animals , Auditory Pathways , Auditory Threshold , Models, Theoretical , Noise , Sound Spectrography , Synaptic Transmission
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...