Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 12 de 12
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Cogn Neurosci ; 3(2): 112-9, 2012.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24168692

ABSTRACT

Patients who suffer from dissociative identity disorder present unique scientific and clinical challenges for psychology and psychiatry. We have been fortunate in working with a patient who-while undergoing functional MRI-can switch rapidly and voluntarily between her main personality (a middle-aged, high-functioning woman) and an alternate personality (a 4-6-year-old girl). A unique task was designed to isolate the processes occurring during the switches between these personalities. Data are from two imaging sessions, conducted months apart, each showing the same activated areas during switches between these personalities. The activated areas include the following: the primary sensory and motor cortex, likely associated with characteristic facial movements made during switching; the nucleus accumbens bilaterally, possibly associated with aspects of reward connected with switching; and prefrontal sites, presumably associated with the executive control involved in the switching of personalities.

2.
Acta Psychol (Amst) ; 107(1-3): 9-42, 2001 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11388144

ABSTRACT

It has long been known that there is some degree of localisation of function in the human brain, as indicated by the effects of traumatic head injury. Work in the middle of the 20th century, notably the direct cortical stimulation of patients during neurosurgery, suggested that the degree and specificity of such localisation of function were far greater than had earlier been imagined. One problem with the data based on lesions and direct stimulation was that the work depended on the study of what were, by definition, damaged brains. During the second half of the 20th century, a collection of relatively non-invasive tools for assessing and localising human brain function in healthy volunteers has led to an explosion of research in what is often termed "Brain Mapping". The present article reviews some of the history associated with these tools, but emphasises the current state of development with speculation about the future.


Subject(s)
Anatomy/history , Brain Mapping , Brain/anatomy & histology , Diagnostic Imaging/history , Neurophysiology/history , Anatomy/trends , Brain/blood supply , Brain/physiology , Diagnostic Imaging/trends , Electroencephalography , Forecasting , Hemodynamics/physiology , History, 20th Century , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Magnetics , Neurophysiology/trends , Tomography, Emission-Computed
3.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 7(1): 15-28, 1999.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9882087

ABSTRACT

Brain activations associated with semantic processing of visual and auditory words were investigated using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). For each form of word presentation, subjects performed two tasks: one semantic, and one nonsemantic. The semantic task was identical for both auditory and visual presentation: single words were presented and subjects determined whether the word was concrete or abstract. In the nonsemantic task for auditory words, subjects determined whether the word had one syllable or multiple syllables. In the nonsemantic task for visual words, subjects determined whether the word was presented in lower case or upper case. There was considerable overlap in where auditory and visual word semantic processing occurred. Visual and auditory semantic tasks both activated the left inferior frontal (BA 45), bilateral anterior prefrontal (BA 10, 46), and left premotor regions (BA 6) and anterior SMA (BA 6, 8). Left posterior temporal (middle temporal and fusiform gyrus) and predominantly right-sided cerebellar activations were observed during the auditory semantic task but were not above threshold during visual word presentation. The data, when averaged across subjects, did not show obligatory activation of left inferior frontal and temporal language areas during nonsemantic word tasks. Individual subjects showed differences in the activation of the inferior frontal region while performing the same task, even though they showed similar response latency and accuracy.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping/methods , Brain/physiology , Language , Reading , Speech , Acoustic Stimulation , Adult , Brain/anatomy & histology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Photic Stimulation
4.
Neuroreport ; 9(15): 3499-502, 1998 Oct 26.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9855306

ABSTRACT

Dextrals with right cerebral hemisphere dominance for language are rare. Eight neurologically intact dextrals underwent BOLD-fMRI while being presented auditory and visual words. Fortuitously, in one subject, right hemisphere activations with visually presented words were seen in the inferior frontal, premotor regions together with predominantly left cerebellar activation. These were a mirror image of activations obtained from the seven other dextrals. Also mirrored was temporal activation from auditory words which extended more posteriorly on the right side than the left. These results showing mirror organization of language were replicated in another scanning session and also by using a second word task. Although rare, mirrored organization of language can occur in normal dextrals without penalizing language function.


Subject(s)
Auditory Cortex/physiology , Dominance, Cerebral/physiology , Language , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Acoustic Stimulation , Adult , Auditory Perception/physiology , Cerebellum/physiology , Frontal Lobe/physiology , Humans , Male , Motor Cortex/physiology , Photic Stimulation , Verbal Learning/physiology , Visual Cortex/physiology
5.
Neuron ; 18(4): 591-8, 1997 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9136768

ABSTRACT

How does voluntary attention to one attribute of a visual stimulus affect the neural processing of that stimulus? We used functional magnetic resonance imaging to examine the attentional modulation of neural activity in the human homolog of the MT-MST complex, which is known to be involved in the processing of visual motion. Using a visual stimulus containing both moving and stationary dots, we found significantly more MT-MST activation when subjects attended to the moving dots than when they attended to the stationary dots, even though the visual stimulus was identical during the two conditions.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Motion Perception/physiology , Visual Cortex/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Eye Movements/physiology , Humans , Middle Aged , Volition
6.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 93(25): 14878-83, 1996 Dec 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8962149

ABSTRACT

Functional neuroimaging studies in human subjects using positron emission tomography or functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) are typically conducted by collecting data over extended time periods that contain many similar trials of a task. Here methods for acquiring fMRI data from single trials of a cognitive task are reported. In experiment one, whole brain fMRI was used to reliably detect single-trial responses in a prefrontal region within single subjects. In experiment two, higher temporal sampling of a more limited spatial field was used to measure temporal offsets between regions. Activation maps produced solely from the single-trial data were comparable to those produced from blocked runs. These findings suggest that single-trial paradigms will be able to exploit the high temporal resolution of fMRI. Such paradigms will provide experimental flexibility and time-resolved data for individual brain regions on a trial-by-trial basis.


Subject(s)
Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Cognitive Science , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male
7.
Percept Psychophys ; 49(5): 448-55, 1991 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2057310

ABSTRACT

The McCollough effect (ME), a long-lasting, pattern-contingent aftereffect in normal human vision, was examined in persons with known deficits in memory. We induced MEs in 11 subjects, 5 patients with various severities of Alzheimer's disease (AD), H.M. (a patient who has global amnesia due to bilateral medial temporal lobectomy and who has been studied for 35 years since his operation), and 5 control subjects. H.M. and the AD patients showed MEs of strength and duration comparable to those of the control subjects. These results demonstrate a dissociation between learning mechanisms that mediate recall and recognition versus mechanisms that mediate the ME. Furthermore, knowledge about the sites of neuropathology in H.M. and in AD are consistent with other sources of evidence implicating early visual areas, especially V1, as a critical locus of the ME.


Subject(s)
Alzheimer Disease/psychology , Amnesia/psychology , Color Perception/physiology , Figural Aftereffect , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Aged , Alzheimer Disease/physiopathology , Amnesia/physiopathology , Cerebral Cortex/physiopathology , Female , Figural Aftereffect/physiology , Humans , Male , Mental Recall/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology
9.
J Opt Soc Am ; 73(9): 1143-8, 1983 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-6631572

ABSTRACT

Following Munsell's bisection procedure [J. Opt. Soc. Am. 23, 394 (1933)], we established a nine-step gray scale in which each step is an equal increment in lightness. We calculated retinal illuminances after intraocular scatter by using the point-spread function of Vos et al. [Vision Res. 16, 215-219 (1976)]. After this correction for intraocular scatter, we find a logarithmic relationship between retinal illuminance and achromatic lightness scales that are determined by the bisection method. Additional bisection experiments with a series of different backgrounds corroborate this result. We find that lightness depends linearly on the logarithm of scatter-corrected retinal illuminance, with different slopes for backgrounds of different lightness. This study also highlights the importance of using scatter-corrected illuminance in any quantitative model of lightness.


Subject(s)
Light , Retina/physiology , Vision, Ocular/physiology , Adult , Humans , Optics and Photonics , Scattering, Radiation
11.
J Opt Soc Am ; 65(3): 343-50, 1975 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1123690

ABSTRACT

The number of cycles in a low-frequency sinusoidal display is a crucial variable in determining the visibility of the display. In particular, the threshold contrast is essentially independent of spatial frequency for these displays. We have extended the above experiments, using more cycles and a variety of targets and observer tasks. The results confirm previous findings; they also show that the type of target or task has little influence. For low-frequency sinusoids that contain up to about 3 cycles, the threshold contrast is determined by the number of cycles. For high-number-of-cycles targets with spatial frequencies above 6-10 cycles per degree, visibility is predominantly dependent on the spatial frequency. The results suggest that the low-frequency decrease in reported MTF's is due to the decrease of the number of cycles used in determining them.


Subject(s)
Space Perception , Visual Acuity , Fourier Analysis , Humans , Photic Stimulation , Time Factors
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...