Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 14 de 14
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Cortex ; 142: 104-121, 2021 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34265734

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: This study investigates the clinical course of recovery of apraxia after left-hemisphere stroke and the underlying neuroanatomical correlates for persisting or recovering deficits in relation to the major processing streams in the network for motor cognition. METHODS: 90 patients were examined during the acute (4.74 ± 2.73 days) and chronic (14.3 ± 15.39 months) stage after left-hemisphere stroke for deficits in meaningless imitation, as well as production and conceptual errors in tool use pantomime. Lesion correlates for persisting or recovering deficits were analyzed with an extension of the non-parametric Brunner-Munzel rank-order test for multi-factorial designs (two-way repeated-measures ANOVA) using acute images. RESULTS: Meaningless imitation and tool use production deficits persisted into the chronic stage. Conceptual errors in tool use pantomime showed an almost complete recovery. Imitation errors persisted after occipitotemporal and superior temporal lesions in the dorso-dorsal stream. Chronic pantomime production errors were related to the supramarginal gyrus, the key structure of the ventro-dorsal stream. More anterior lesions in the ventro-dorsal stream (ventral premotor cortex) were additionally associated with poor recovery of production errors in pantomime. Conceptual errors in pantomime after temporal and supramarginal gyrus lesions persisted into the chronic stage. However, they resolved completely when related to angular gyrus or insular lesions. CONCLUSION: The diverging courses of recovery in different apraxia tasks can be related to different mechanisms. Critical lesions to key structures of the network or entrance areas of the processing streams lead to persisting deficits in the corresponding tasks. Contrary, lesions located outside the core network but inducing a temporary network dysfunction allow good recovery e.g., of conceptual errors in pantomime. The identification of lesion correlates for different long-term recovery patterns in apraxia might also allow early clinical prediction of the course of recovery.


Subject(s)
Apraxias , Stroke , Apraxias/diagnostic imaging , Apraxias/etiology , Humans , Imitative Behavior , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Parietal Lobe , Stroke/complications , Stroke/diagnostic imaging
2.
Sci Rep ; 11(1): 4478, 2021 02 24.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33627742

ABSTRACT

The concepts of brain reserve and cognitive reserve were recently suggested as valuable predictors of stroke outcome. To test this hypothesis, we used age, years of education and lesion size as clinically feasible coarse proxies of brain reserve, cognitive reserve, and the extent of stroke pathology correspondingly. Linear and logistic regression models were used to predict cognitive outcome (Montreal Cognitive Assessment) and stroke-induced impairment and disability (NIH Stroke Scale; modified Rankin Score) in a sample of 104 chronic stroke patients carefully controlled for potential confounds. Results revealed 46% of explained variance for cognitive outcome (p < 0.001) and yielded a significant three-way interaction: Larger lesions did not lead to cognitive impairment in younger patients with higher education, but did so in younger patients with lower education. Conversely, even small lesions led to poor cognitive outcome in older patients with lower education, but didn't in older patients with higher education. We observed comparable three-way interactions for clinical scores of stroke-induced impairment and disability both in the acute and chronic stroke phase. In line with the hypothesis, years of education conjointly with age moderated effects of lesion on stroke outcome. This non-additive effect of cognitive reserve suggests its post-stroke protective impact on stroke outcome.


Subject(s)
Cognitive Reserve/physiology , Stroke/physiopathology , Cognitive Dysfunction/physiopathology , Educational Status , Female , Humans , Male , Mental Status and Dementia Tests , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Severity of Illness Index
3.
Neuroradiology ; 62(12): 1637-1648, 2020 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32691076

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: Precise segmentation of brain lesions is essential for neurological research. Specifically, resection volume estimates can aid in the assessment of residual postoperative tissue, e.g. following surgery for glioma. Furthermore, behavioral lesion-symptom mapping in epilepsy relies on accurate delineation of surgical lesions. We sought to determine whether semi- and fully automatic segmentation methods can be applied to resected brain areas and which approach provides the most accurate and cost-efficient results. METHODS: We compared a semi-automatic (ITK-SNAP) with a fully automatic (lesion_GNB) method for segmentation of resected brain areas in terms of accuracy with manual segmentation serving as reference. Additionally, we evaluated processing times of all three methods. We used T1w, MRI-data of epilepsy patients (n = 27; 11 m; mean age 39 years, range 16-69) who underwent temporal lobe resections (17 left). RESULTS: The semi-automatic approach yielded superior accuracy (p < 0.001) with a median Dice similarity coefficient (mDSC) of 0.78 and a median average Hausdorff distance (maHD) of 0.44 compared with the fully automatic approach (mDSC 0.58, maHD 1.32). There was no significant difference between the median percent volume difference of the two approaches (p > 0.05). Manual segmentation required more human input (30.41 min/subject) and therefore inferring significantly higher costs than semi- (3.27 min/subject) or fully automatic approaches (labor and cost approaching zero). CONCLUSION: Semi-automatic segmentation offers the most accurate results in resected brain areas with a moderate amount of human input, thus representing a viable alternative compared with manual segmentation, especially for studies with large patient cohorts.


Subject(s)
Epilepsy/diagnostic imaging , Epilepsy/surgery , Image Interpretation, Computer-Assisted/methods , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Retrospective Studies
4.
J Neurol ; 266(10): 2495-2504, 2019 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31254064

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: Although post-stroke cognitive deficit can significantly limit patient independence and social re-integration, clinical routine predictors for this condition are lacking. 'Cognitive reserve' limits the detrimental effects of slowly developing neurodegeneration. We aimed to determine whether comparable effects also exist in acute stroke. Using 'years of education' as a proxy, we investigated whether cognitive reserve beneficially influences cognitive performance and disability after stroke, whilst controlling for age and lesion size as measure of stroke pathology. METHODS: Within the first week of ischemic right hemisphere stroke, 36 patients were assessed for alertness, working memory, executive functions, spatial neglect, global cognition and motor deficit at 4.9 ± 2.1 days post-stroke, in addition to routine clinical tests (NIH Stroke Scale, modified Rankin Scale on admission < 24 h post-stroke and at discharge 9.5 ± 4.7 days post-stroke). The impact of education was assessed using partial correlation analysis adjusted for lesion size, age, and the time interval between stroke and assessment. To validate our results, we compared groups with similar age and lesion load, but different education levels. RESULTS: In the acute stroke phase, years of education predicted both severity of education independent (alertness) and education dependent (working memory, executive functions, global cognition) cognitive deficits and disability (modified Rankin Scale). Spatial neglect seemed to be independent. INTERPRETATION: Proxies of cognitive reserve should be considered in stroke research as early as in the acute stroke phase. Cognitive reserve contributes to inter-individual variability in the initial severity of cognitive deficits and disability in acute stroke, and may suggest individualised rehabilitation strategies.


Subject(s)
Brain Ischemia/physiopathology , Cognitive Dysfunction/physiopathology , Cognitive Reserve/physiology , Severity of Illness Index , Stroke/physiopathology , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Brain Ischemia/complications , Cognitive Dysfunction/etiology , Cohort Studies , Educational Status , Female , Humans , Infarction, Middle Cerebral Artery/complications , Infarction, Middle Cerebral Artery/physiopathology , Male , Middle Aged , Stroke/complications
5.
Cortex ; 120: 1-21, 2019 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31220613

ABSTRACT

Behavioral deficits after stroke like apraxia can be related to structural lesions and to a functional state of the underlying network - three factors, reciprocally influencing each other. Combining lesion data, behavioral performance and passive functional activation of the network-of-interest, this study aims to disentangle those mutual influences and to identify 1) activation patterns associated with the presence or absence of acute apraxia in tool-associated actions and 2) the specific impact of lesion location on those activation patterns. Brain activity of 48 patients (63.31 ± 13.68 years, 35 male) was assessed in a fMRI paradigm with observation of tool-related actions during the acute phase after first-ever left-hemispheric stroke (4.83 ± 2.04 days). Behavioral assessment of apraxia in tool-related tasks was obtained independently. Brain activation was compared between patients versus healthy controls and between patient with versus without apraxia. Interaction effects of lesion location (frontal vs parietal) and behavioral performance (apraxia vs no apraxia) were assessed in a 2 × 2 factorial design. Action observation activated the ventro-dorsal parts of the network for cognitive motor function; activation was globally downregulated after stroke. Apraxic compared to non-apraxic patients showed relatively increased activity in bilateral posterior middle temporal gyrus and middle frontal gyrus/superior frontal sulcus. Altered activation occurred in regions for tool-related cognition, corroborating known functions of the ventro-dorsal and ventral streams for praxis, and comprised domain-general areas, functionally related to cognitive control. The interaction analyses revealed different levels of activation in the left anterior middle temporal gyrus in the ventral stream in apraxic patients with frontal compared to parietal lesions, suggesting a modulation of network activation in relation to behavioral performance and lesion location as separate factors. By detecting apraxia-specific activation patterns modulated by lesion location, this study underlines the necessity to combine structural lesion information, behavioral parameters and functional activation to comprehensively examine cognitive functions in acute stroke patients.


Subject(s)
Apraxias/diagnostic imaging , Stroke/diagnostic imaging , Acute Disease , Aged , Apraxias/etiology , Brain Mapping , Cognition , Factor Analysis, Statistical , Female , Frontal Lobe/diagnostic imaging , Frontal Lobe/physiopathology , Functional Laterality , Humans , Imitative Behavior , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged , Neural Pathways/diagnostic imaging , Neural Pathways/physiopathology , Neuropsychological Tests , Observation , Parietal Lobe/diagnostic imaging , Parietal Lobe/physiopathology , Stroke/complications
6.
Neuroimage Clin ; 23: 101840, 2019.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31108458

ABSTRACT

Previous lesion studies suggest that semantic and phonological fluency are differentially subserved by distinct brain regions in the left temporal and the left frontal cortex, respectively. However, as of yet, this often implied double dissociation has not been explicitly investigated due to mainly two reasons: (i) the lack of sufficiently large samples of brain-lesioned patients that underwent assessment of the two fluency variants and (ii) the lack of tools to assess interactions in factorial analyses of non-normally distributed behavioral data. In addition, previous studies did not control for task resource artifacts potentially introduced by the generally higher task difficulty of phonological compared to semantic fluency. We addressed these issues by task-difficulty adjusted assessment of semantic and phonological fluency in 85 chronic patients with ischemic stroke of the left middle cerebral artery. For classical region-based lesion-behavior mapping patients were grouped with respect to their primary lesion location. Building on the extension of the non-parametric Brunner-Munzel rank-order test to multi-factorial designs, ANOVA-type analyses revealed a significant two-way interaction for cue type (semantic vs. phonological) by lesion location (left temporal vs. left frontal vs. other as stroke control group). Subsequent contrast analyses further confirmed the proposed double dissociation by demonstrating that (i) compared to stroke controls, left temporal lesions led to significant impairments in semantic but not in phonological fluency, whereas left frontal lesions led to significant impairments in phonological but not in semantic fluency, and that (ii) patients with frontal lesions showed significantly poorer performance in phonological than in semantic fluency, whereas patients with temporal lesions showed significantly poorer performance in semantic than in phonological fluency. The anatomical specificity of these findings was further assessed in voxel-based lesion-behavior mapping analyses using the multi-factorial extension of the Brunner-Munzel test. Voxel-wise ANOVA-type analyses identified circumscribed parts of left inferior frontal gyrus and left superior and middle temporal gyrus that significantly double-dissociated with respect to their differential contribution to phonological and semantic fluency, respectively. Furthermore, a main effect of lesion with significant impairments in both fluency types was found in left inferior frontal regions adjacent to but not overlapping with those showing the differential effect for phonological fluency. The present study hence not only provides first explicit evidence for the anatomical double dissociation in verbal fluency at the group level but also clearly underlines that its formulation constitutes an oversimplification as parts of left frontal cortex appear to contribute to both semantic and phonological fluency.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping/methods , Frontal Lobe/diagnostic imaging , Phonetics , Semantics , Stroke/diagnostic imaging , Temporal Lobe/diagnostic imaging , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Female , Frontal Lobe/physiology , Humans , Language Tests , Male , Middle Aged , Stroke/psychology , Temporal Lobe/physiology , Young Adult
7.
Cereb Cortex ; 28(2): 474-492, 2018 02 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27909000

ABSTRACT

Imitation of tool-use gestures (transitive; e.g., hammering) and communicative emblems (intransitive; e.g., waving goodbye) is frequently impaired after left-hemispheric lesions. We aimed 1) to identify lesions related to deficient transitive or intransitive gestures, 2) to delineate regions associated with distinct error types (e.g., hand configuration, kinematics), and 3) to compare imitation to previous data on pantomimed and actual tool use. Of note, 156 patients (64.3 ± 14.6 years; 56 female) with first-ever left-hemispheric ischemic stroke were prospectively examined 4.8 ± 2.0 days after symptom onset. Lesions were delineated on magnetic resonance imaging scans for voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping. First, while inferior-parietal lesions affected both gesture types, specific associations emerged between intransitive gesture deficits and anterior temporal damage and between transitive gesture deficits and premotor and occipito-parietal lesions. Second, impaired hand configurations were related to anterior intraparietal damage, hand/wrist-orientation errors to premotor lesions, and kinematic errors to inferior-parietal/occipito-temporal lesions. Third, premotor lesions impacted more on transitive imitation compared with actual tool use, pantomimed and actual tool use were more susceptible to lesioned insular cortex and subjacent white matter. In summary, transitive and intransitive gestures differentially rely on ventro-dorsal and ventral streams due to higher demands on temporo-spatial processing (transitive) or stronger reliance on semantic information (intransitive), respectively.


Subject(s)
Cerebral Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Communication , Gestures , Imitative Behavior/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Stroke/diagnostic imaging , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Cerebral Cortex/physiopathology , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Male , Middle Aged , Prospective Studies , Stroke/physiopathology
8.
Neuropsychologia ; 99: 148-155, 2017 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28257876

ABSTRACT

Verbal fluency for semantic categories and phonological letters is frequently applied to studies of language and executive functions. Despite its popularity, it is still debated whether measures of semantic and phonological fluency reflect the same or distinct sets of cognitive processes. Word generation in the two task variants is believed to involve different types of search processes. Findings from the lesion and neuroimaging literature further suggest a stronger reliance of phonological and semantic fluency on frontal and temporal brain areas, respectively. This evidence for differential cognitive and neural contributions is, however, strongly challenged by findings from factor analyses, which have consistently yielded only one explanatory factor. As all previous factor-analytical approaches were based on very small item sets, this apparent discrepancy may be due to methodological limitations. In this study, we therefore applied a German version of the verbal fluency task with 8 semantic (i.e. categories) and 8 phonological items (i.e. letters). An exploratory factor analysis with oblique rotation in N=69 healthy young adults indeed revealed a two-factor solution with markedly different loadings for semantic and phonological items. This pattern was corroborated by a confirmatory factor analysis in a sample of N=174 stroke patients. As results from both samples also revealed a substantial portion of common variance between the semantic and phonological factor, the present data further demonstrate that semantic and phonological verbal fluency are based on clearly distinct but also on shared sets of cognitive processes.


Subject(s)
Cognition , Phonetics , Semantics , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Factor Analysis, Statistical , Female , Humans , Language Tests , Male , Middle Aged , Stroke/psychology , Young Adult
9.
Exp Brain Res ; 235(1): 83-95, 2017 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27637595

ABSTRACT

Visual neglect after left-hemispheric lesion is thought to be less frequent, less severe, and shorter lived than visuospatial attention deficits resulting from right-hemispheric lesions. However, reports exist opposing this assumption, and it is unclear how these findings fit into the current theories of visuospatial processing. Furthermore, only little is known about the exact structure-function relationship between visuospatial attention deficits and left-hemispheric stroke. We investigated neglect in 121 patients with acute left-hemispheric ischemic stroke by following clinical development from within the first 24 h of stroke onset until hospital discharge. Visuospatial attention deficits occurred in 17.4 % (n = 21). Voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping associated visual neglect to the right with lesion in the left superior and middle temporal gyrus, temporal pole, frontal operculum, and insula. Neglect severity, captured by the Center of Cancellation Score of the Bells test, was associated with lesion in the left anterior temporal lobe and the left frontal operculum. The left-hemispheric lesion pattern of neglect thus involves areas of the ventral attention system and partly mirrors the critical regions of the right hemisphere known to be associated with neglect. Based on our prospective analysis on a large cohort of patients with left-hemispheric stroke, this study shows that in a remarkable number of patients, the left hemisphere essentially contributes to an intact representation of space and clarifies the impact of the distinct left-hemispheric structures involved in visuospatial processing.


Subject(s)
Brain Injuries/etiology , Brain Mapping , Functional Laterality/physiology , Perceptual Disorders/etiology , Stroke/complications , Visual Perception/physiology , Aged , Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/diagnostic imaging , Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity/etiology , Brain Injuries/diagnostic imaging , Female , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged , Neurologic Examination , Neuropsychological Tests , Perceptual Disorders/diagnostic imaging , Retrospective Studies , Severity of Illness Index , Statistics, Nonparametric , Stroke/diagnostic imaging , Visual Field Tests
10.
Cereb Cortex ; 27(8): 4139-4152, 2017 08 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27497285

ABSTRACT

The study aimed to elucidate areas involved in recognizing tool-associated actions, and to characterize the relationship between recognition and active performance of tool use.We performed voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping in a prospective cohort of 98 acute left-hemisphere ischemic stroke patients (68 male, age mean ± standard deviation, 65 ± 13 years; examination 4.4 ± 2 days post-stroke). In a video-based test, patients distinguished correct tool-related actions from actions with spatio-temporal (incorrect grip, kinematics, or tool orientation) or conceptual errors (incorrect tool-recipient matching, e.g., spreading jam on toast with a paintbrush). Moreover, spatio-temporal and conceptual errors were determined during actual tool use.Deficient spatio-temporal error discrimination followed lesions within a dorsal network in which the inferior parietal lobule (IPL) and the lateral temporal cortex (sLTC) were specifically relevant for assessing functional hand postures and kinematics, respectively. Conversely, impaired recognition of conceptual errors resulted from damage to ventral stream regions including anterior temporal lobe. Furthermore, LTC and IPL lesions impacted differently on action recognition and active tool use, respectively.In summary, recognition of tool-associated actions relies on a componential network. Our study particularly highlights the dissociable roles of LTC and IPL for the recognition of action kinematics and functional hand postures, respectively.


Subject(s)
Brain Ischemia/physiopathology , Brain/physiopathology , Motion Perception/physiology , Motor Skills/physiology , Recognition, Psychology/physiology , Stroke/physiopathology , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Apraxias/diagnostic imaging , Apraxias/etiology , Apraxias/physiopathology , Apraxias/psychology , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Brain Ischemia/diagnostic imaging , Brain Ischemia/psychology , Female , Hand/physiopathology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Prospective Studies , Stroke/diagnostic imaging , Stroke/psychology , Young Adult
11.
Arch Clin Neuropsychol ; 31(7): 738-753, 2016 Nov 22.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27590302

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: Planning as a prototypical executive function is frequently compromised in clinical samples. Analyses of rule breaking during performance on tower tasks are highly informative for clinical inferences on planning deficits, but are as yet only available for manual task versions. Therefore, the present study investigated whether valid rule-break information can also be gained using a computerized Tower of London (TOL) version. METHOD: For patients with stroke (N = 60), Parkinson's disease (PD; N = 51), and Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI; N = 29), rule breaks (e.g., selecting a blocked ball) and online-planning errors (re-considering the next move) on the TOL-Freiburg (TOL-F) were compared to that of matched healthy controls, and diagnostic accuracy for these measures was assessed. RESULTS: Stroke and PD patients committed significantly more rule breaks and online-planning errors than matched controls, with this group difference being significantly greater for rule breaks. For MCI patients, only a trend for elevated numbers of rule breaks emerged. In all patients, rule breaks and errors increased as a function of problem complexity. Diagnostic accuracy was the highest for PD and lowest for MCI patients, with both rule breaks and errors demonstrating moderate sensitivity but higher specificity across all samples. DISCUSSION: Direct attempts to break the TOL-F's task rules are particularly informative of clinical impairments in planning ability. Similar to findings from studies employing manual versions, using a computerized tower task revealed consistent rule break and error patterns across patients. Thus, computerized tower tasks can yield useful clinical information on rule-breaking behavior during planning in clinical populations.

12.
Cereb Cortex ; 26(9): 3754-71, 2016 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26271112

ABSTRACT

Impaired tool use despite preserved basic motor functions occurs after stroke in the context of apraxia, a cognitive motor disorder. To elucidate the neuroanatomical underpinnings of different tool use deficits, prospective behavioral assessments of 136 acute left-hemisphere stroke patients were combined with lesion delineation on magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) images for voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping. Deficits affecting both the selection of the appropriate recipient for a given tool (ToolSelect, e.g., choosing the nail for the hammer), and the performance of the typical tool-associated action (ToolUse, e.g., hammering in the nail) were associated with ventro-dorsal stream lesions, particularly within inferior parietal lobule. However, ToolSelect compared with ToolUse deficits were specifically related to damage within ventral stream regions including anterior temporal lobe. Additional retrospective error dichotomization based on the videotaped performances of ToolUse revealed that spatio-temporal errors (movement errors) were mainly caused by inferior parietal damage adjacent to the intraparietal sulcus while content errors, that is, perplexity, unrecognizable, or semantically incorrect movements, resulted from lesions within supramarginal gyrus and superior temporal lobe. In summary, our results suggest that in the use of tools, conceptual and production-related aspects can be differentiated and are implemented in anatomically distinct streams.


Subject(s)
Apraxias/pathology , Apraxias/physiopathology , Psychomotor Performance , Stroke/pathology , Stroke/physiopathology , Temporal Lobe/pathology , Temporal Lobe/physiopathology , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Apraxias/etiology , Concept Formation , Female , Humans , Intention , Male , Middle Aged , Neuronal Plasticity , Stroke/complications
13.
Neuropsychologia ; 75: 646-55, 2015 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26197091

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: Executive deficits are frequent sequelae of neurological and psychiatric disorders, but their adequate neuropsychological assessment is still a matter of contention, given that executive tasks draw on a multitude of cognitive processes that are often not sufficiently specified. In line with this, results on psychometric properties of the Tower of London, a task measuring planning ability as a prototypical executive function, are equivocal and furthermore lacking completely for adult clinical populations. METHODS: We used a structurally balanced item set implemented in the Tower of London (Freiburg version, TOL-F) that accounts for major determinants of problem difficulty beyond the commonly used minimum number of moves to solution. Split-half reliability, internal consistency, and criterion-related concurrent validity of TOL-F accuracy were assessed in patients with stroke (N = 60), Parkinson syndrome (N = 51), and mild cognitive impairment (N = 29), and healthy adults (N = 155). RESULTS: Across samples, mean split-half and lower-bound indices of reliability of accuracy scores were adequate (r ≥ .7) or higher. Compared to a subset of healthy controls matched for age, sex, and education levels, deficits in planning accuracy emerged for all three clinical samples. CONCLUSIONS: Based on consistently adequate reliability and a good criterion-related validity of accuracy scores, the TOL-F demonstrates its utility for testing planning ability in clinical samples and healthy adults. Using item sets systematically accounting for several determinants of task difficulty can thus significantly enhance the contended reliability of executive tasks and provide an opportunity to resolve the underspecification of cognitive processes contributing to executive functioning in health and disease.


Subject(s)
Executive Function , Neuropsychological Tests , Problem Solving , Aged , Cognitive Dysfunction/diagnosis , Cognitive Dysfunction/psychology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests/statistics & numerical data , Parkinsonian Disorders/diagnosis , Parkinsonian Disorders/psychology , Psychometrics , Reproducibility of Results , Stroke/diagnosis , Stroke/psychology
14.
Epilepsy Behav ; 44: 17-22, 2015 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25597528

ABSTRACT

Unilateral hippocampal atrophy (HA) is considered as a precursor of mesial temporal sclerosis (MTS) in some patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. However, in other cases, it has been suggested that HA without MTS may constitute a distinct epileptic entity. Hippocampal atrophy without MTS was defined as HA without T2-weighted hyperintensity, loss of internal architecture, or associated lesion seen on the MRI data. To date, no study has focused on the cognitive pattern of children with epilepsy with HA without MTS. The objectives of the present study were to characterize the cognitive profile of these children and to investigate the presence (or the absence) of material-specific memory deficits in these young patients, as found in patients with MTS. Toward this end, 16 young patients with epilepsy with either left or right HA without MTS completed a set of neuropsychological tests, assessing overall intelligence, verbal memory and nonverbal memory, and some aspects of attention and executive functions. Results showed normal intellectual functioning without specific memory deficits in these patients. Furthermore, comparison between patients with left HA and patients with right HA failed to reveal a material-specific lateralized memory pattern. Instead, attention and executive functions were found to be impaired in most patients. These results suggest that HA may constitute a distinct epileptic entity, and this information may help health-care providers initiate appropriate and timely interventions.


Subject(s)
Epilepsy, Temporal Lobe/psychology , Hippocampus/pathology , Adolescent , Age of Onset , Atrophy , Attention , Child , Electroencephalography , Epilepsy, Temporal Lobe/complications , Female , Humans , Intelligence Tests , Learning Disabilities/etiology , Learning Disabilities/psychology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Memory , Memory Disorders/etiology , Memory Disorders/psychology , Neuropsychological Tests , Sclerosis , Verbal Learning , Young Adult
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...