Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 13 de 13
Filter
1.
Disasters ; 48(1): e12593, 2024 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37227427

ABSTRACT

Liminal periods of disaster solidarity in the aftermath of disaster are a common experience of many survivors. These periods have a specifically ethical component in that people spontaneously engage in collective, altruistic action and magnanimously expand their ethical focus beyond normative social distinctions and hierarchies. Inevitably, however, such solidarity seems to wane, and people return to pre-disaster patterns of interaction. Nevertheless, some individuals move beyond opportune acts of assistance to more extensive reorganisations of their lives during the recovery period and reshape their ethical commitments in new and durable directions. These individuals help make visible marginalised 'others' and draw collaborators to share new ethical visions. Based on observational and interview data collected after Hurricane María (2017) in a mountainous Puerto Rican municipality and employing the framework of virtue ethics, this paper examines the differential effects of disaster solidarity on survivors' ethical responses and the different contributions these make to society.


Subject(s)
Cyclonic Storms , Disasters , Ethics , Survivors , Humans , Altruism , Hispanic or Latino , Puerto Rico
2.
J Pragmat ; 168: 56-68, 2020 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32943809

ABSTRACT

Interlocutors hold one another accountable for knowing certain information about themselves (their roles, activities, and memories) and for keeping track of information in the current conversational exchange. When speakers have trouble with this expectation, they themselves work to repair the breach, often doing a memory search, or when unsuccessful they provide an account (e.g. "I don't remember"). Memory searches (like word searches) are observable, interactional accomplishments. Speakers disengage with their interlocutors (look away), produce hesitation markers, take repeated pauses, engage in pre-positioned and post-positioned repairs, make epistemic assessments, and on finding an answer, re-engage with their interlocutors (look back). For their part, interlocutors comply with the search by not interrupting and continuing to yield the floor. At progressively severe stages of Alzheimer's disease, individuals exhibit increasingly labored memory searches that often trail off into non-answers, until at the latest stages, they eschew the search and (almost smoothly) provide either grammatically appropriate but wrong and improbable answers or give answers to previous questions on now closed topics. With data from the clinical administration of a disease staging instrument (Clinical Dementia Rating) this article examines the inexorable loss of epistemic responsibility as a key discursive dynamic in the progression of the disease.

3.
Memory ; 23(2): 278-90, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24524255

ABSTRACT

The fading affect bias (FAB) refers to the negative affect associated with autobiographical events fading faster than the positive affect associated with such events, a reliable and valid valence effect established by researchers in the USA. The present study examined the idea that the FAB is a ubiquitous emotion regulating phenomenon in autobiographical memory that is present in people from a variety of cultures. We tested for evidence of the FAB by sampling more than 2400 autobiographical event descriptions from 562 participants in 10 cultures around the world. Using variations on a common method, each sample evidenced a FAB: positive affect faded slower than negative affect did. Results suggest that in tandem with local norms and customs, the FAB may foster recovery from negative life events and promote the retention of the positive emotions, within and outside of the USA. We discuss these findings in the context of Keltner and Haidt's levels of analysis theory of emotion and culture.


Subject(s)
Affect , Cross-Cultural Comparison , Memory, Episodic , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Female , Humans , Male , Mental Recall , Middle Aged , Young Adult
4.
J Am Geriatr Soc ; 59(4): 628-36, 2011 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21480837

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: To understand how people differentiate normal memory loss from Alzheimer's disease (AD) by investigating cultural models of these conditions. DESIGN: Ethnographic interviews followed by a survey. Cultural consensus analysis was used to test for the presence of group models, derive the "culturally correct" set of beliefs, and compare models of normal memory loss and AD. SETTING: Chicago, Illinois. PARTICIPANTS: One hundred eight individuals from local neighborhoods: African Americans, Mexican Americans, and refugees and immigrants from the former Soviet Union. MEASUREMENTS: Participants responded to yes-or-no questions about the nature and causes of normal memory loss and AD and provided information on ethnicity, age, sex, acculturation, and experience with AD. RESULTS: Groups held a common model of AD as a brain-based disease reflecting irreversible cognitive decline. Higher levels of acculturation predicted greater knowledge of AD. Russian speakers favored biological over psychological models of the disease. Groups also held a common model of normal memory loss, including the important belief that "normal" forgetting involves eventual recall of the forgotten material. CONCLUSION: Popular models of memory loss and AD confirm that patients and clinicians are speaking the same "language" in their discussions of memory loss and AD. Nevertheless, the presence of coherent models of memory loss and AD, and the unequal distribution of that knowledge across groups, suggests that clinicians should include wider circles of patients' families and friends in their consultations. These results frame knowledge as distributed across social groups rather than simply the possession of individual minds.


Subject(s)
Alzheimer Disease/complications , Black or African American , Emigrants and Immigrants , Memory Disorders/ethnology , Memory/physiology , Mexican Americans , Refugees , Adult , Age Distribution , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Alzheimer Disease/ethnology , Alzheimer Disease/psychology , Female , Humans , Illinois/epidemiology , Incidence , Male , Memory Disorders/etiology , Memory Disorders/psychology , Middle Aged , Sex Distribution , Surveys and Questionnaires , USSR/ethnology
5.
J Cross Cult Gerontol ; 24(2): 115-20, 2009 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19408109
6.
J Cross Cult Gerontol ; 24(2): 157-79, 2009 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19184621

ABSTRACT

This research focuses on patterns of English proficiency and use-of-English among older immigrants living in linguistically concentrated, ethnic neighborhoods. A sample (n = 60) of older Puerto Ricans, who moved from the island to the mainland in their twenties, were divided into English proficiency groups (fluent, high intermediate, low intermediate) via the Adult Language Assessment Scales. Participants then provided self-ratings of their English proficiency (understanding, speaking, reading, and writing), their use of English in social domains (language spoken with own-family, in-laws, spouse, children, neighbors, and workmates), and their use of English in private psychological domains (language of talking to oneself, counting, writing notes to oneself, thinking, dreaming, praying, and expressing feelings). Finally, all participants completed the Puerto Rican Bicultural Scale. Results show a cohort of immigrant elders whose first language is protected by their ethnic neighborhoods but whose domestic and private lives are increasingly permeated by English. In particular, children emerge as powerful forces of language socialization in English for their parents. Further, there are important individual differences by level of proficiency, with a lowest proficiency group that is less acculturated, lower in socioeconomic status, and even more linguistically isolated than groups with higher proficiency. In essence, level of second language proficiency is a potent source of intracultural variation. Methodologically, the paper makes the important point that self-rated patterns of language use are consistent with scores on formal measures of proficiency. The paper also provides empirical verification of the logic of dividing language use into external, social speech and internal, psychological speech.


Subject(s)
Communication , Interpersonal Relations , Multilingualism , Residence Characteristics , Acculturation , Aged , Emigrants and Immigrants , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Puerto Rico/ethnology , Surveys and Questionnaires , United States
7.
Res Gerontol Nurs ; 2(3): 214-24, 2009 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20078011

ABSTRACT

This article provides a systematic review of linguistically and culturally adapted versions (translations) of the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) in languages other than English and Spanish. Adapted versions were found via database search (PubMed, CINAHL, PsycINFO, and Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts). Source information (where available) is provided for each instrument. To facilitate comparison of the instruments, we developed an assessment framework that provides an organizational structure for the assessment of the quality of translation (linguistic and cultural), psychometric properties, educational considerations, and quality of field testing. The search resulted in 20 articles reporting on 15 language versions of the MMSE.


Subject(s)
Mental Status Schedule/standards , Multilingualism , Translations , Aged , Cross-Cultural Comparison , Cultural Competency , Geriatric Assessment , Humans , Linguistics , Nursing Assessment , Nursing Evaluation Research , Psychometrics , Reproducibility of Results , United States
8.
J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci ; 63(6): S385-93, 2008 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19092048

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: Freelisting is a brief, paper-and-pencil technique in which participants make lists of items that they believe belong in a particular domain. Where cultural domains are shared, as for young and old in the same society, subtle intracultural differences may be difficult to detect. This article presents a series of techniques for revealing and describing this intracultural variation in freelisted data among young versus old age groups. METHODS: Older (N = 30) and younger (N = 31) Mexicans in Mexico City made freelists in four quotidian domains: animals, emotions, illnesses, and gendered occupations. RESULT: We used minimum residual factor analysis (consensus analysis) to establish domain coherence and assess overall consensus concerning contents of the domains. We established subvariation within the overall consensus by comparing levels of observed versus predicted inter-informant agreement. Results showed divergent patterns of inter-informant agreement between young and old participants across domains. Qualitative examination of items with higher salience for young versus old revealed age differences consistent with prior findings in each domain. DISCUSSION: The concatenation of these techniques renders freelisting an accessible, easily administered tool for probing age and group differences in cultural domains.


Subject(s)
Aging/psychology , Anthropology, Cultural/methods , Cultural Characteristics , Data Collection/methods , Human Development , Adolescent , Adult , Age Factors , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Consensus , Female , Humans , Male , Mexico/ethnology , Middle Aged , Semantics
9.
Memory ; 15(5): 536-47, 2007 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17613796

ABSTRACT

Although the underlying mechanics of autobiographical memory may be identical across cultures, the processing of information differs. Undergraduates from Japan, Turkey, and the USA rated 30 autobiographical memories on 15 phenomenological and cognitive properties. Mean values were similar across cultures, with means from the Japanese sample being lower on most measures but higher on belief in the accuracy of their memories. Correlations within individuals were also similar across cultures, with correlations from the Turkish sample being higher between measures of language and measures of recollection and belief. For all three cultures, in multiple regression analyses, measures of recollection were predicted by visual imagery, auditory imagery, and emotions, whereas measures of belief were predicted by knowledge of the setting. These results show subtle cultural differences in the experience of remembering.


Subject(s)
Autobiographies as Topic , Cognition/physiology , Memory , Adolescent , Adult , Cross-Cultural Comparison , Emotions , Humans , Japan , Regression Analysis , Turkey , United States
10.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc ; 12(3): 391-9, 2006 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16903131

ABSTRACT

Adaptations of the National Adult Reading Test (NART) for assessing premorbid intelligence in languages other than English requires (a) generating word-items that are rare and do not follow grapheme-to-phoneme mappings common in that language, and (b) subsequent validation against a cognitive battery normed on the population of interest. Such tests exist for Italy, France, Spain, and Argentina, all normed against national versions of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. Given the varieties of Spanish spoken in the United States, the adaptation of the Spanish Word Accentuation Test (WAT) requires re-validating the original word list, plus possible new items, against a cognitive battery that has been normed on Spanish-speakers from many countries. This study reports the generation of 55 additional words and revalidation in a sample of 80 older, Spanish-dominant immigrants. The Batería Woodcock-Muñoz Revisada (BWM-R), normed on Spanish speakers from six countries and five U.S. states, was used to establish criterion validity. The original WAT word list accounted for 77% of the variance in the BWM-R and 58% of the variance in Ravens Colored Progressive Matrices, suggesting that the unmodified list possesses adequate predictive validity as an indicator of intelligence. Regression equations are provided for estimating BWM-R and Ravens scores from WAT scores.


Subject(s)
Alzheimer Disease/ethnology , Dementia/epidemiology , Emigration and Immigration/statistics & numerical data , Hispanic or Latino/statistics & numerical data , Intelligence , Language , Vocabulary , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Alzheimer Disease/diagnosis , Dementia/diagnosis , Feasibility Studies , Humans , Multilingualism , Neuropsychological Tests , Phonetics , Predictive Value of Tests , Severity of Illness Index , United States/epidemiology
11.
Memory ; 12(6): 715-21, 2004 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15724360

ABSTRACT

A total of 30 undergraduates recalled the same 20 autobiographical memories at two sessions separated by 2 weeks. At each session they dated their memories and rated them on 18 properties commonly studied in autobiographical memory experiments. Individuals showed moderate stability in their ratings on the 18 scales (r approximately .5), with consistency of dating being much higher (r = .96). There was more stability in the individuals' average rating on each scale (r approximately .8), even when the averages were calculated on different memories in the different sessions. The results are consistent with a constructive view of autobiographical memory, in which stable individual differences in cognitive style are important.


Subject(s)
Memory , Adolescent , Adult , Cognition , Cues , Female , Humans , Male , Mental Recall , Psychological Tests , Time Factors
12.
Mem Cognit ; 31(6): 887-901, 2003 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14651297

ABSTRACT

In three experiments, undergraduates rated autobiographical memories on scales derived from existing theories of memory. In multiple regression analyses, ratings of the degree to which subjects recollected (i.e., relived) their memories were predicted by visual imagery, auditory imagery, and emotions, whereas ratings of belief in the accuracy of their memories were predicted by knowledge of the setting. Recollection was predicted equally well in between- and within-subjects analyses, but belief consistently had smaller correlations and multiple regression predictions between subjects; individual differences in the cognitive scales that we measured could not account well for individual differences in belief. In contrast, measures of mood (Beck Depression Index) and dissociation (Dissociative Experience Scale) added predictive value for belief, but not for recollection. We also found that highly relived memories almost always had strong visual images and that remember/know judgments made on autobiographical memories were more closely related to belief than to recollection.


Subject(s)
Autobiographies as Topic , Culture , Mental Recall , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Male
13.
Memory ; 10(1): 45-54, 2002 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11747575

ABSTRACT

Thirty years after fleeing from Poland to Denmark, 20 immigrants were enlisted in a study of bilingual autobiographical memory. Ten "early immigrators" averaged 24 years old at the time of immigration, and ten "late immigrators" averaged 34 years old at immigration. Although all 20 had spent 30 years in Denmark, early immigrators reported more current inner speech behaviours in Danish, whereas late immigrators showed more use of Polish. Both groups displayed proportionally more numerous autobiographical retrievals that were reported as coming to them internally in Polish (vs Danish) for the decades prior to immigration and more in Danish (vs Polish) after immigration. We propose a culture- and language-specific shaping of semantic and conceptual stores that underpins autobiographical and world knowledge.


Subject(s)
Ethnicity/psychology , Memory , Multilingualism , Adult , Age Factors , Aged , Denmark , Emigration and Immigration , Humans , Middle Aged , Poland/ethnology , Semantics , Thinking
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...