Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 15 de 15
Filter
Add more filters











Publication year range
1.
PLoS One ; 15(5): e0232813, 2020.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32428002

ABSTRACT

Worldwide, nearly 3 million people die every year because of being overweight or obese. Although obesity is a metabolic disease, behavioral aspects are important in its etiology. Hunger changes the rewarding potential of food in normal-weight controls. In obesity, impairments related to reward processing are present, but it is not clear whether these are due to mental disorders more common among this population. Therefore, in this pilot study, we aimed at investigating whether fasting influence mood reactivity to reward in people with obesity. Women with obesity (n = 11, all mentally healthy) and normal weight controls (n = 17) were compared on a computerized monetary reward task (the wheel of fortune), using self-reports of mood and affect (e.g., PANAS and mood evaluation during the task) as dependent variables. This task was done in 2 satiety conditions, during fasting and after eating. Partially, in line with our expectation of a reduced affect and mood reactivity to monetary reward in participants with obesity accentuated by fasting, our results indicated a significant within-group difference across time (before and after the task), with monetary gains significantly improving positive affect in healthy controls (p>0.001), but not in individuals with obesity (p = 0.32). There were no significant between-group differences in positive affect before (p = 0.328) and after (p = 0.70) the task. In addition, women with obesity, compared to controls, reported more negative affect in general (p < 0.05) and less mood reactivity during the task in response to risky gains (p < 0.001) than healthy controls. The latter was independent of the level of satiety. These preliminary results suggest an impairment in mood reactivity to monetary reward in women with obesity which is not connected to the fasting state. Increasing the reinforcing potential of rewards other than food in obesity may be one target of intervention in order to verify if that could reduce overeating.


Subject(s)
Affect/physiology , Hunger/physiology , Mental Disorders/physiopathology , Obesity/psychology , Adult , Body Mass Index , Fasting/physiology , Fasting/psychology , Female , Food/adverse effects , Humans , Male , Mental Disorders/psychology , Middle Aged , Obesity/physiopathology , Overweight/physiopathology , Overweight/psychology , Pilot Projects , Reward
2.
Eur Eat Disord Rev ; 28(1): 46-54, 2020 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31713283

ABSTRACT

Anorexia nervosa (AN) is an eating disorder characterized by a low food intake and often exceeding exercise, leading to a particularly low body × weight proportion. Patients with AN usually report less hunger than healthy controls. Endogenous endocannabinoids (eCBs), specifically the anandamide, have been associated to hunger, as a meal initiator, but research regarding AN and eCB and inconclusive. In this pilot study, we investigated plasma levels of eCB in inpatients with AN during fasting and after eating, both during the acute AN phase and after weight recovery. After an 8-hr fasting period, blood sample was collected from all participants. After that, participants were given a muffin test meal. Blood samples for the investigation of endogenous eCBs anandamide (N-arachidonoylethanolamide [AEA]) and 2-arachidonoylglycerol (2-AG) were then collected after 120 and 240 min. Participants were only allowed to eat and drink what was offered them during the research. AN reported less hunger than controls during fasting and at the end of the experiment. Also, plasma levels of AEA were significantly smaller in AN in comparison with controls in all time points. No significant difference was found for 2-AG plasma levels. After recovery, no significant difference was found for eCB levels. These findings could be interpreted as an AEA deregulation in AN before and after food intake, which persists after weight recovery. These findings may have implications to the pharmacological treatment of AN and to relapse occurring in the disorder.


Subject(s)
Anorexia Nervosa/blood , Anorexia Nervosa/therapy , Endocannabinoids/blood , Fasting/blood , Humans , Pilot Projects , Young Adult
3.
Front Psychol ; 10: 2492, 2019.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31749750

ABSTRACT

Impaired decision-making under conditions of uncertainty seems to contribute to the expression and maintenance of anorexia nervosa (AN), but it is not clear whether this impairment is a disease state that would remit with treatment, or a persisting trait in patients with AN. To examine this question, a longitudinal study was conducted in 12 female inpatients with AN (age M = 22.2, SE = 1.36), before (Time-1) and after reaching a body mass index of >17.5 kg/m2 (Time-2). Intolerance of uncertainty (IU) was assessed via a decision-making task, the wheel of fortune (WOF). Weight gain at Time-2 was accompanied with significant changes in uncertainty-related performance compared to Time-1 [(Time × Uncertainty), p < 0.05]. At Time-1, reaction times (RTs) varied in function of uncertainty, while at Time-2, uncertainty did not modulate RTs. These findings support a change in decision-making under uncertainty with successful weight-rehabilitation in AN. While IU was present in underweight patients, it became non-significant after weight restoration.

4.
Appetite ; 134: 26-33, 2019 03 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30562563

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Previous studies using neuroimaging and behavioral measures reported altered reward processing in anorexia nervosa (AN). In addition, anhedonia states are frequently reported in AN, potentially due to the physiological stress produced by the permanent starvation. We investigated the effect of fasting and satiety on mood and reaction times to monetary rewards in AN patients and healthy controls. METHODS: Twenty-four participants with acute AN (BMI 14.4 (11.9-15.5) Kg/m2) and 17 age and gender matched healthy, normal weight subjects (HW) (BMI 21.8 (18.9-24.9) Kg/m2) performed a reward task (the wheel of fortune) involving uncertain (50/50 probability of winning high and low rewards), safe and risky (30/70 and 10/90 probabilities) categories in fasted (after an 8-h fasting period) and fed (after intake of a standardized meal) states. Data analysis was done with linear mixed models. RESULTS: AN reacted slower than HW when maximum uncertainty (50/50) was involved. Positive mood in response to winning was higher when fasting especially for HW, while negative mood in response to not winning was higher in the fed state for both groups. Still, HW were more reactive than AN to not winning a highly predictable monetary reward (10/90 safe). CONCLUSION: The data on the reaction times indicate an impaired motor response to uncertainty in AN. Mood reactivity to winning a monetary reward does not seem to be impaired in AN, however, our results suggest that negative mood in response to not winning is less adaptive in AN. Implications to clinical psychotherapy are discussed.


Subject(s)
Anorexia Nervosa/psychology , Hunger , Reward , Adolescent , Adult , Body Mass Index , Fasting , Female , Humans , Satiation , Young Adult
5.
PLoS One ; 13(1): e0190809, 2018.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29300752

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Previous research has demonstrated that the neuropeptide oxytocin modulates social behaviors and reduces anxiety. However, effects of oxytocin on startle reactivity, a well-validated measure of defense system activation related to fear and anxiety, have been inconsistent. Here we investigated the influence of oxytocin on startle reactivity with particular focus on the role of trait anxiety. METHODS: Forty-four healthy male participants attended two experimental sessions. They received intranasal oxytocin (24 IU) in one session and placebo in the other. Startle probes were presented in combination with pictures of social and non-social content. Eye-blink startle magnitude was measured by electromyography over the musculus orbicularis oculi in response to 95 dB noise bursts. Participants were assigned to groups of high vs. low trait anxiety based on their scores on the trait form of the Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI). RESULTS: A significant interaction effect of oxytocin with STAI confirmed that trait anxiety moderated the effect of oxytocin on startle reactivity. Post-hoc tests indicated that for participants with elevated trait anxiety, oxytocin increased startle magnitude, particularly when watching non-social pictures, while this was not the case for participants with low trait anxiety. CONCLUSION: Results indicate that effects of oxytocin on defense system activation depend on individual differences in trait anxiety. Trait anxiety may be an important moderator variable that should be considered in human studies on oxytocin effects.


Subject(s)
Anxiety/physiopathology , Oxytocin/physiology , Reflex, Startle/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Blinking/drug effects , Blinking/physiology , Cross-Over Studies , Double-Blind Method , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Oxytocin/pharmacology , Personality/physiology , Reflex, Startle/drug effects , Young Adult
6.
Appetite ; 114: 368-373, 2017 07 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28392423

ABSTRACT

This study investigates the subjective estimation of weight gain in patients with anorexia nervosa (AN) when being confronted with food cues both in a general (self-unrelated) and in an intent-to-eat (self-related) condition. Looking at the presentation of different snack pictures with different nutrition values (high-low calories), AN patients (N = 24) and age-matched healthy women (N = 27) estimated the weight gain when they imagined eating the presented portions of snack pictures once a day in addition to the normal daily nutrition in the following two conditions: 1) a general condition without specific additional instruction, 2) an intent-to-eat condition, in which they were instructed to imagine that they would eat the snack themselves. Compared to healthy women, patients with AN estimated a higher weight gain only in the intent-to-eat condition, i.e. when they imagined eating the snacks themselves, but not in the general, not self-related condition. In the patient group, mean estimations of weight gain were associated with the "drive for thinness". This study suggests cognitive abnormalities related to the effects of food intake on the weight gain in AN, and that these cognitive anomalies could be related to the fear of gaining weight, one central symptom of AN. It appears that the self-reflective disturbed cognition, rather than the general cognition, could be the main driver underlying anorexia and that the overestimation of the energetic content of food is related to the drive for thinness.


Subject(s)
Anorexia Nervosa/physiopathology , Anxiety/etiology , Cognitive Dissonance , Energy Intake , Health Knowledge, Attitudes, Practice , Models, Psychological , Overweight/prevention & control , Adolescent , Adult , Anorexia Nervosa/psychology , Anorexia Nervosa/therapy , Anxiety/psychology , Body Mass Index , Combined Modality Therapy , Cues , Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders , Female , Humans , Overweight/psychology , Pilot Projects , Psychiatric Status Rating Scales , Serving Size , Snacks/psychology , Switzerland , Thinness/etiology , Thinness/prevention & control , Thinness/psychology , Weight Gain , Young Adult
7.
J Pain ; 17(12): 1325-1333, 2016 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27641312

ABSTRACT

There is growing evidence that fear-learning abnormalities are involved in the development of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and chronic pain. More than 50% of PTSD patients suffer from chronic pain. This study aimed to examine the role of fear-learning deficits in the link between pain perception and PTSD. We included 19 subjects with PTSD and 21 age- and sex-matched healthy control subjects in a fear-conditioning experiment. The conditioned stimulus (CS) consisted of visual signs flashed upon a screen in front of each subject. The unconditioned stimulus was either a low or high temperature impulse delivered through a thermal contact thermode on the subjects' hand. A designation of 'CS-' was assigned to CS always followed by nonpainful low-temperature stimuli; a designation of 'CS+' was given to CS that were randomly followed by either a low or a more painful high temperature. Skin conductance was used as a physiological marker of fear. In healthy control subjects, CS+ induced more fear than CS-, and a low-temperature stimulus induced less subjective pain after CS- than after CS+. PTSD subjects failed to demonstrate such adaptive conditioning. Fear ratings after CS presentation were significantly higher in the PTSD group than in the control group. There were significant interaction effects between group and the type of CS on fear and pain ratings. Fear-learning deficits are a potentially promising, specific psychopathological factor in altered pain perception associated with PTSD. Deficits in safety learning may increase fear and, consequently, pain sensations. These findings may contribute to elucidating the pathogenesis behind the highly prevalent comorbidity that exists between PTSD and pain disorders, and to developing new treatments. PERSPECTIVE: This study provides new insights into the pathogenesis of chronic pain in patients with PTSD. The findings may help to develop new treatment strategies for this highly prevalent comorbidity in PTSD.


Subject(s)
Chronic Pain/etiology , Conditioning, Classical , Fear/psychology , Learning Disabilities/etiology , Pain Perception/physiology , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/complications , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/psychology , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Chronic Pain/psychology , Female , Humans , Learning Disabilities/epidemiology , Male , Middle Aged , Pain Threshold/physiology , Physical Stimulation/adverse effects , Psychometrics , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/epidemiology , Young Adult
8.
Psychiatry Res ; 240: 144-150, 2016 06 30.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27107667

ABSTRACT

Impairments in classical fear conditioning and deficits in discriminative learning are observed in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). However, it is unknown whether similar impairments can be found with types of discriminative learning other than classical conditioning, such as evaluative conditioning (EC), in which the valence of a stimulus can be transferred to other stimuli. In this study, we investigated whether EC is also influenced by traumatic experiences independently of presence of PTSD. We tested 14 accident survivors with remitted PTSD, 14 survivors without PTSD, and 16 non-trauma controls. We used behavioral measures, psychophysiological indicators, and subjective ratings for tasks. General effects of learning were observed across groups and conditioning/extinction. Trauma controls had slower reaction times (RTs) to the aversive conditioned stimulus compared to appetitive conditioned and neutral stimuli, as well as slower RTs and increased accuracy during conditioning than during extinction. Remitted PTSD participants showed opposite results, demonstrating decreased accuracy and slower RTs during conditioning as compared to during extinction. No discriminative effect was found in the non-trauma controls and the remitted PTSD participants. These results suggest that a traumatic experience influences EC, and that this influence differs between individuals who have and have not developed PTSD after traumatic exposure.


Subject(s)
Accidents/psychology , Conditioning, Psychological/physiology , Extinction, Psychological/physiology , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/psychology , Survivors/psychology , Adult , Case-Control Studies , Conditioning, Classical/physiology , Fear/psychology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Reaction Time , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/physiopathology , Time
9.
Brain Behav ; 5(8): e00357, 2015 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26357590

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Recent evidence suggests blunted responses to rewarding stimuli in patients with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). However, it is not clear whether these alterations in reward processing normalize in remitted PTSD patients. METHODS: We tested behavioral and physiological responses to monetary reward in a spatial memory task in 13 accident survivors with remitted PTSD, 14 accident survivors who never had PTSD, and 16 nontrauma-exposed subjects. All accident survivors were recruited from two samples of severely physically injured patients, who had participated in previous prospective studies on the incidence of PTSD after accidental injury approximately 10 years ago. Reaction time, accuracy, skin conductance responses, and self-reported mood were assessed during the task. RESULTS: Accident survivors who never had PTSD and nontrauma exposed controls reported significantly higher positive mood in the reinforced versus nonreinforced condition (P < 0.045 and P < 0.001, respectively), while there was no effect of reinforcement in remitted PTSD subjects. CONCLUSIONS: Our findings suggest an alteration of the reward system in remitted PTSD. Further research is needed to investigate whether altered reward processing is a residual characteristic in PTSD after remission of symptoms or, alternatively, a preexisting risk factor for the development of PTSD after a traumatic event.


Subject(s)
Reinforcement, Psychology , Reward , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/psychology , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/therapy , Behavior Rating Scale , Case-Control Studies , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Prospective Studies , Psychiatric Status Rating Scales , Psychophysiology/methods , Recovery of Function , Spatial Memory
10.
J Pers Disord ; 29(6): 809-27, 2015 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25710735

ABSTRACT

Earlier studies have demonstrated emotional overreactions to affective visual stimuli in patients with borderline personality disorder (BPD). However, contradictory findings regarding hyper- versus hyporeactivity have been reported for peripheral physiological measures. In order to extend previous results, the authors investigated emotional reactivity and long-term habituation in the acoustic modality. Twenty-two female BPD patients and 19 female nonclinical controls listened to emotionally negative, neutral, and positive sounds in two identical sessions. Heart rate, skin conductance, zygomaticus/corrugator muscle, and self-reported valence/arousal responses were measured. BPD patients showed weaker skin conductance responses to negative sounds than controls. The elevated zygomaticus activity in response to positive sounds observed in controls was absent in BPD patients, and BPD patients assigned lower valence ratings to positive sounds than controls. In Session 2, patients recognized fewer positive sounds than controls. Across both groups, physiological measures habituated between sessions. These findings add to growing evidence toward partial affective hyporeactivity in BPD.


Subject(s)
Affect/physiology , Arousal/physiology , Attention/physiology , Auditory Perception/physiology , Borderline Personality Disorder/diagnosis , Adult , Borderline Personality Disorder/psychology , Case-Control Studies , Electrocardiography , Electromyography , Emotions , Female , Galvanic Skin Response/physiology , Heart Rate/physiology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Substance-Related Disorders/psychology
11.
Psychiatry Res ; 210(2): 570-4, 2013 Dec 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23870491

ABSTRACT

An exaggerated startle response is one of the core hyperarousal symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Heightened startle eye-blink magnitude and reduced habituation of this response in PTSD patients have been reported in several studies. However, it is unclear whether this is an enduring characteristic of individuals vulnerable for PTSD or to which degree trauma-exposed individuals who do not develop PTSD also show exaggerated startle. Thirteen accident survivors with remitted PTSD, 12 trauma controls, and 16 non-trauma controls were examined. Four measures of startle reactivity were analyzed in response to 15 bursts of white noise (95 dB, 50 ms): eye-blink magnitude, eye-blink onset latency, skin conductance response, and heart rate response. The eye-blink reflex was measured over the left musculus orbicularis oculi. Reactivity and habituation were analyzed using linear mixed models. Remitted PTSD subjects did not differ from non-trauma controls regarding any of the startle reactivity or habituation measures. Unexpectedly, trauma controls showed larger eye-blink magnitude than non-trauma controls. These results suggest that the exaggerated startle response disappears after remission from PTSD. Further, they suggest that psychologically resilient trauma survivors might show a PTSD-like pattern of exaggerated physiological startle even many years after a traumatic event.


Subject(s)
Reflex, Startle/physiology , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/psychology , Stress, Psychological/psychology , Acoustic Stimulation , Adult , Blinking/physiology , Electromyography , Female , Galvanic Skin Response/physiology , Habituation, Psychophysiologic/physiology , Heart Rate/physiology , Humans , Linear Models , Male , Middle Aged , Resilience, Psychological , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/diagnosis , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/physiopathology , Stress, Psychological/diagnosis , Survivors/psychology , Trauma Severity Indices
12.
J Nerv Ment Dis ; 201(6): 471-7, 2013 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23686156

ABSTRACT

Little is known about the influence of particular characteristics of childhood maltreatment, such as developmental stage, relationship to the perpetrator, and nature of the trauma, on adult psychopathology. The effects of childhood maltreatment were assessed in adult psychiatric patients (N = 287) using self-rating scales and diagnostic checklists. Maltreatment was strongly associated with dissociation. This relationship was observed for all childhood developmental stages and was strongest when the perpetrator was outside the family. Dissociation was more strongly correlated with childhood emotional abuse and sexual harassment than with sexual or physical abuse. Childhood sexual abuse was found to be associated with symptoms of posttraumatic stress. The findings suggest that dissociation is a relatively specific consequence of childhood maltreatment that is largely independent of the familial relationship to the perpetrator or the child's developmental stage.


Subject(s)
Adult Survivors of Child Abuse/psychology , Depression/etiology , Dissociative Disorders/etiology , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/etiology , Adolescent , Adult , Adult Survivors of Child Abuse/statistics & numerical data , Age Factors , Aged , Checklist , Child , Child Abuse, Sexual/psychology , Child Development , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Psychiatric Status Rating Scales , Surveys and Questionnaires , Young Adult
13.
Appetite ; 63: 42-7, 2013 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23276722

ABSTRACT

Pathological concern regarding one's weight and weight gain is a crucial feature of anorexia nervosa. Consequently, anorexia nervosa patients often claim that they are uncertain regarding the amount of food they should eat. The present study investigated whether individuals with anorexia nervosa show an altered estimation of meal portion sizes and whether this estimation is modulated by an intent-to-eat instruction (where patients are asked to imagine having to eat the presented meal), meal type and meal portion size. Twenty-four women with anorexia nervosa and 27 healthy women estimated, using a visual analogue scale, the size of six different portions of three different meals, with and without intent-to-eat instructions. Subjects with anorexia nervosa estimated the size of small and medium meal portions (but not large meal servings) as being significantly larger, compared to estimates of healthy controls. The overestimation of small meal portions by anorexia nervosa subjects was significantly greater in the intent-to-eat, compared to general, condition. These findings suggest that disturbed perceptions associated with anorexia nervosa not only include interoceptive awareness (i.e., body weight and shape), but also extend to external disorder-related objects such as meal portion size. Specific therapeutic interventions, such as training regarding meal portion evaluation, could address these difficulties.


Subject(s)
Anorexia Nervosa/prevention & control , Eating/psychology , Feeding Behavior/psychology , Meals/psychology , Adolescent , Adult , Anorexia Nervosa/psychology , Case-Control Studies , Female , Humans , Hunger/physiology , Neuropsychological Tests , Self Report , Weight Gain , Young Adult
15.
J Trauma Dissociation ; 11(3): 337-57, 2010.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20603767

ABSTRACT

The present study investigated the validity of the German version of the Somatoform Dissociation Questionnaire (SDQ-20), a scale designed to measure somatoform dissociative symptoms. Somatoform dissociation involves physical manifestations of a dissociation of the personality and is considered a unique entity in the phenomenological spectrum of dissociation. The validity and reliability of the German version of the SDQ-20 was examined using a sample of 225 patients with (n = 39) and without dissociative disorders who were recruited from several in- and outpatient psychiatric clinics. Patients were assessed using structured diagnostic interviews; diagnostic checklists; and self-rating scales for dissociation, and posttraumatic stress. Patients with dissociative disorders reported significantly more (p < .001) somatoform dissociative symptoms than patients without dissociative disorders (criterion validity). Significant correlations (p < .001) were found between scores of somatoform dissociation, psychoform dissociation, posttraumatic stress symptoms, and traumatic childhood experiences (construct validity). Reliability was corroborated by a Cronbach's alpha coefficient of .91 and a test-retest correlation of .89. A component factor analysis suggested unidimensionality of the SDQ-20. In conclusion, the psychometric properties and cross-cultural validity of the German version of the SDQ-20 are excellent. Our results form the basis for the further study of somatoform dissociation in German-speaking populations.


Subject(s)
Cross-Cultural Comparison , Dissociative Disorders/diagnosis , Personality Inventory/statistics & numerical data , Somatoform Disorders/diagnosis , Surveys and Questionnaires , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Child Abuse/psychology , Comorbidity , Dissociative Disorders/psychology , Female , Germany , Humans , Life Change Events , Male , Mental Disorders/diagnosis , Mental Disorders/psychology , Middle Aged , Psychometrics/statistics & numerical data , Reproducibility of Results , Somatoform Disorders/psychology , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/diagnosis , Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic/psychology , Switzerland , Translating , Young Adult
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL