Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 8 de 8
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Neuroimage ; 181: 301-313, 2018 11 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29990584

ABSTRACT

Meditation training can improve mood and emotion regulation, yet the neural mechanisms of these affective changes have yet to be fully elucidated. We evaluated the impact of long- and short-term mindfulness meditation training on the amygdala response to emotional pictures in a healthy, non-clinical population of adults using blood-oxygen level dependent functional magnetic resonance imaging. Long-term meditators (N = 30, 16 female) had 9081 h of lifetime practice on average, primarily in mindfulness meditation. Short-term training consisted of an 8-week Mindfulness- Based Stress Reduction course (N = 32, 22 female), which was compared to an active control condition (N = 35, 19 female) in a randomized controlled trial. Meditation training was associated with less amygdala reactivity to positive pictures relative to controls, but there were no group differences in response to negative pictures. Reductions in reactivity to negative stimuli may require more practice experience or concentrated practice, as hours of retreat practice in long-term meditators was associated with lower amygdala reactivity to negative pictures - yet we did not see this relationship for practice time with MBSR. Short-term training, compared to the control intervention, also led to increased functional connectivity between the amygdala and a region implicated in emotion regulation - ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC) - during affective pictures. Thus, meditation training may improve affective responding through reduced amygdala reactivity, and heightened amygdala-VMPFC connectivity during affective stimuli may reflect a potential mechanism by which MBSR exerts salutary effects on emotion regulation ability.


Subject(s)
Amygdala/physiology , Emotions/physiology , Functional Neuroimaging/methods , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Meditation , Mindfulness , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology , Adult , Amygdala/diagnostic imaging , Connectome/methods , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Prefrontal Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Time Factors
2.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci ; 12(11): 1701-1710, 2017 11 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28981837

ABSTRACT

Empathy, the ability to understand others' emotions, can occur through perspective taking and experience sharing. Neural systems active when adults empathize include regions underlying perspective taking (e.g. medial prefrontal cortex; MPFC) and experience sharing (e.g. inferior parietal lobule; IPL). It is unknown whether adolescents utilize networks implicated in both experience sharing and perspective taking when accurately empathizing. This question is critical given the importance of accurately understanding others' emotions for developing and maintaining adaptive peer relationships during adolescence. We extend the literature on empathy in adolescence by determining the neural basis of empathic accuracy, a behavioral assay of empathy that does not bias participants toward the exclusive use of perspective taking or experience sharing. Participants (N = 155, aged 11.1-15.5 years) watched videos of 'targets' describing emotional events and continuously rated the targets' emotions during functional magnetic resonance imaging scanning. Empathic accuracy related to activation in regions underlying perspective taking (MPFC, temporoparietal junction and superior temporal sulcus), while activation in regions underlying experience sharing (IPL, anterior cingulate cortex and anterior insula) related to lower empathic accuracy. These results provide novel insight into the neural basis of empathic accuracy in adolescence and suggest that perspective taking processes may be effective for increasing empathy.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Emotions/physiology , Empathy/physiology , Interpersonal Relations , Adolescent , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Child , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male
3.
Sci Rep ; 6: 27533, 2016 06 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27272738

ABSTRACT

Respiration rate is known to correlate with aspects of psychological well-being, and attention to respiration is a central component of mindfulness meditation training. Both traditional contemplative systems and recent empirical evidence support an association between formal mindfulness practice and decreased respiration rate. However, the question of whether long-term mindfulness training is associated with stable, generalized changes in respiration has yet to be directly investigated. We analyzed respiration patterns across multiple time points, separated by two months or more, in a group of long-term mindfulness meditation practitioners (LTMs, n = 31) and a matched group of non-meditators (Controls, n = 38). On average, LTMs showed slower baseline respiration rate (RR) than Controls. Among LTMs, greater practice experience was associated with slower RR, independently of age and gender. Furthermore, this association was specific to intensive retreat practice, and was not seen for routine daily practice. Full days of meditation practice did not produce detectable changes in baseline RR, suggesting distal rather than immediate effects. All effects were independent of physiological characteristics including height, weight, body-mass index and waist-hip ratio. We discuss implications for continued study of the long-term effects of mindfulness training on health and well-being.


Subject(s)
Meditation/psychology , Mindfulness , Respiratory Rate/physiology , Teaching/psychology , Adult , Aged , Attention/physiology , Awareness/physiology , Body Mass Index , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Plethysmography , Rest/physiology , Waist-Hip Ratio
4.
Psychoneuroendocrinology ; 68: 117-25, 2016 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26970711

ABSTRACT

Psychological stress is a major contributor to symptom exacerbation across many chronic inflammatory conditions and can acutely provoke increases in inflammation in healthy individuals. With the rise in rates of inflammation-related medical conditions, evidence for behavioral approaches that reduce stress reactivity is of value. Here, we compare 31 experienced meditators, with an average of approximately 9000 lifetime hours of meditation practice (M age=51years) to an age- and sex-matched control group (n=37; M age=48years) on measures of stress- and inflammatory responsivity, and measures of psychological health. The Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) was used to induce psychological stress and a neurogenic inflammatory response was produced using topical application of capsaicin cream to forearm skin. Size of the capsaicin-induced flare response and increase in salivary cortisol and alpha amylase were used to quantify the magnitude of inflammatory and stress responses, respectively. Results show that experienced meditators have lower TSST-evoked cortisol (62.62±2.52 vs. 70.38±2.33; p<.05) and perceived stress (4.18±.41 vs. 5.56±.30; p<.01), as well as a smaller neurogenic inflammatory response (81.55±4.6 vs. 96.76±4.26; p<.05), compared to the control group. Moreover, experienced meditators reported higher levels of psychological factors associated with wellbeing and resilience. These results suggest that the long-term practice of meditation may reduce stress reactivity and could be of therapeutic benefit in chronic inflammatory conditions characterized by neurogenic inflammation.


Subject(s)
Meditation/methods , Meditation/psychology , Stress, Psychological/metabolism , Adult , Female , Humans , Hydrocortisone/metabolism , Inflammation/metabolism , Male , Middle Aged , Randomized Controlled Trials as Topic , Research Design , Saliva/metabolism , Stress, Psychological/psychology , alpha-Amylases/metabolism
5.
Psychol Assess ; 28(8): 1009-14, 2016 08.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26460893

ABSTRACT

The current study attempted a rigorous test of the construct validity of a widely used self-report measure of dispositional mindfulness, the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ), within the context of an active controlled randomized trial (n = 130). The trial included three arms: mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR), an active control condition that did not include instruction in mindfulness meditation (Health Enhancement Program [HEP]), and a waitlist control condition. Partial evidence for the convergent validity of the FFMQ was shown in correlations at baseline between FFMQ facets and measures of psychological symptoms and psychological well-being. In addition, facets of the FFMQ were shown to increase over the course of an MBSR intervention relative to a waitlist control condition. However, the FFMQ failed to show discriminant validity. Specifically, facets of the FFMQ were shown to increase over the course of the HEP intervention relative to the waitlist control condition. MBSR and HEP, in contrast, did not differ in changes in FFMQ score over time. Implications of these findings for the measurement and theory of mindfulness and MBSR are discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record


Subject(s)
Health Promotion/methods , Meditation/methods , Mental Health , Mindfulness/methods , Surveys and Questionnaires , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Meditation/psychology , Middle Aged , Reproducibility of Results
6.
Neuropsychologia ; 77: 288-97, 2015 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26359718

ABSTRACT

A fast growing literature of multisensory emotion integration notwithstanding, the chemical senses, intimately associated with emotion, have been largely overlooked. Moreover, an ecologically highly relevant principle of "inverse effectiveness", rendering maximal integration efficacy with impoverished sensory input, remains to be assessed in emotion integration. Presenting minute, subthreshold negative (vs. neutral) cues in faces and odors, we demonstrated olfactory-visual emotion integration in improved emotion detection (especially among individuals with weaker perception of unimodal negative cues) and response enhancement in the amygdala. Moreover, while perceptual gain for visual negative emotion involved the posterior superior temporal sulcus/pSTS, perceptual gain for olfactory negative emotion engaged both the associative olfactory (orbitofrontal) cortex and amygdala. Dynamic causal modeling (DCM) analysis of fMRI timeseries further revealed connectivity strengthening among these areas during crossmodal emotion integration. That multisensory (but not low-level unisensory) areas exhibited both enhanced response and region-to-region coupling favors a top-down (vs. bottom-up) account for olfactory-visual emotion integration. Current findings thus confirm the involvement of multisensory convergence areas, while highlighting unique characteristics of olfaction-related integration. Furthermore, successful crossmodal binding of subthreshold aversive cues not only supports the principle of "inverse effectiveness" in emotion integration but also accentuates the automatic, unconscious quality of crossmodal emotion synthesis.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Emotions/physiology , Olfactory Perception/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Physiological/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Brain Mapping , Cues , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Neural Pathways/physiology , Neuropsychological Tests , Physical Stimulation , Respiration , Young Adult
7.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci ; 9(2): 176-81, 2014 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23160815

ABSTRACT

An individual's affective style is influenced by many things, including the manner in which an individual responds to an emotional challenge. Emotional response is composed of a number of factors, two of which are the initial reactivity to an emotional stimulus and the subsequent recovery once the stimulus terminates or ceases to be relevant. However, most neuroimaging studies examining emotional processing in humans focus on the magnitude of initial reactivity to a stimulus rather than the prolonged response. In this study, we use functional magnetic resonance imaging to study the time course of amygdala activity in healthy adults in response to presentation of negative images. We split the amygdala time course into an initial reactivity period and a recovery period beginning after the offset of the stimulus. We find that initial reactivity in the amygdala does not predict trait measures of affective style. Conversely, amygdala recovery shows predictive power such that slower amygdala recovery from negative images predicts greater trait neuroticism, in addition to lower levels of likability of a set of social stimuli (neutral faces). These data underscore the importance of taking into account temporal dynamics when studying affective processing using neuroimaging.


Subject(s)
Amygdala/physiology , Emotions/physiology , Personality/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Aged , Anxiety Disorders , Face , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Neuroticism , Photic Stimulation , Time Factors
8.
Neuroimage ; 49(1): 603-11, 2010 Jan 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19619665

ABSTRACT

Sensitivity, specificity, and reproducibility are vital to interpret neuroscientific results from functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) experiments. Here we examine the scan-rescan reliability of the percent signal change (PSC) and parameters estimated using Dynamic Causal Modeling (DCM) in scans taken in the same scan session, less than 5 min apart. We find fair to good reliability of PSC in regions that are involved with the task, and fair to excellent reliability with DCM. Also, the DCM analysis uncovers group differences that were not present in the analysis of PSC, which implies that DCM may be more sensitive to the nuances of signal changes in fMRI data.


Subject(s)
Magnetic Resonance Imaging/statistics & numerical data , Models, Neurological , Models, Statistical , Acoustic Stimulation , Adolescent , Adult , Algorithms , Female , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Male , Middle Aged , Photic Stimulation , Reproducibility of Results , Young Adult
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...