Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 24
Filter
1.
Neurology ; 70(22): 2024-35, 2008 May 27.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18322263

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To assess the safety of galantamine in subjects with mild cognitive impairment (MCI), the ability of galantamine to benefit cognition and global functioning in subjects with MCI, and the ability of galantamine to delay conversion to dementia. METHODS: In two studies, 2,048 subjects, 990 in Study 1 and 1,058 in Study 2, with a Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) = 0.5, CDR memory score > or =0.5, without dementia were randomized to double-blind galantamine (16-24 mg/day) or placebo for 24 months. Primary efficacy endpoint at month 24 was number (%) of subjects who converted from MCI to dementia (CDR > or = 1.0). RESULTS: There were no differences between galantamine and placebo in 24-month conversion rates (Study 1: 22.9% [galantamine] vs 22.6% [placebo], p = 0.146; Study 2: 25.4% [galantamine] vs 31.2% [placebo], p = 0.619). Mean CDR-sum of boxes declined less with galantamine than placebo at 12 and 24 months in Study 1 (p = 0.024 [12 months] and p = 0.028 [24 months]), but not in Study 2 (p = 0.662 [12 months] and p = 0.056 [24 months]). Digit Symbol Substitution Test scores improved with galantamine in Study 1 at 12 months and in Study 2 at 24 months (Study 1: p = 0.009 [month 12] and p = 0.079 [Month 24]; Study 2: p = 0.154 [month 12] and p = 0.020 [month 24]). The most frequently reported adverse event was nausea (galantamine, 29%; placebo, 10%). Serious AEs occurred in 19% of each group. Mortality of the cohort after retrospectively determining the status of subjects (98.3%) at 24 months was 1.4% (galantamine) and 0.3% (placebo); RR (95% CI), 1.70 (1.00, 2.90). CONCLUSIONS: Galantamine failed to significantly influence conversion to dementia. Galantamine was generally well tolerated. Whereas recorded mortality was greater in the galantamine group than in the placebo group in the original per-protocol assessment, a post hoc analysis of the cohort was consistent with no increased risk.


Subject(s)
Cognition Disorders/drug therapy , Cognition Disorders/psychology , Galantamine/adverse effects , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Alzheimer Disease/mortality , Alzheimer Disease/prevention & control , Cognition Disorders/mortality , Cohort Studies , Double-Blind Method , Female , Galantamine/therapeutic use , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Retrospective Studies
2.
Genes Brain Behav ; 6(3): 209-15, 2007 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16764677

ABSTRACT

Exaggerated pupil response to dilute tropicamide has been suggested as an early biological marker for Alzheimer's disease. The current study links apolipoprotein E (ApoE) allelic variability to the magnitude of pupil response in a sample of community-dwelling elderly without a diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease or dementia. Possession of an epsilon 4 allele influences both the likelihood of exhibiting an exaggerated pupil response above a predetermined cut-off (13% above baseline diameter) and the absolute overall magnitude of the response. Allelic variability was also shown to correlate with cognitive impairments in memory and attention. The data in this study further elucidate the nature of the biological bond between an exaggerated pupil response and the pathology of Alzheimer's disease. ApoE allelic variability is probably linked to pupil response through its influence on tau hyperphosphorylation. The early Alzheimer's pathology seen in the Edinger-Westphal area of cranial nerve III, a major centre for pupil control, is primarily tau-based with significant cell loss in this nucleus leading to central denervation hypersensitivity even in elderly who are clinically silent but who have early pathology.


Subject(s)
Apolipoprotein E4/genetics , Cognition Disorders/genetics , Cognition/physiology , Muscarinic Antagonists/pharmacology , Pupil/drug effects , Tropicamide/pharmacology , Administration, Topical , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Alzheimer Disease/diagnosis , Alzheimer Disease/genetics , Analysis of Variance , Attention/physiology , Dilatation/methods , Dose-Response Relationship, Drug , Female , Gene Dosage , Genetic Predisposition to Disease , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Muscarinic Antagonists/administration & dosage , Mydriatics/administration & dosage , Ophthalmic Solutions , Reference Values , Tropicamide/administration & dosage
3.
Water Res ; 38(3): 507-16, 2004 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14723918

ABSTRACT

Growing concern over the ecological consequence of phosphorus (P) enrichment in freshwater wetlands has elicited considerable debate over the concentration of water column P associated with eutrophication. In the oligotrophic Everglades, the displacement of native communities by enriched ones is widespread and has occurred at sites experiencing only minimal elevations in P input. To help define regulatory criteria for P inputs to the Everglades, we constructed an experiment that mimics P input to the natural system by continuously delivering P at concentrations elevated 5, 15 and 30 microgl(-1) above ambient to 100-m long flow-through channels. We compared patterns of P accumulation in the water, periphyton, detritus and soils among the channel treatments and also along a 16 km transect from an enriched canal that inflows to the interior of the same marsh. Water column TP and SRP were unrelated to input TP concentration in both the experiment and the marsh transect. However, concentrations of TP in periphyton mats were significantly elevated at all levels of experimental enrichment and as far as 2 km downstream from water inputs into the marsh. Elevated periphyton TP was associated with significant loss of periphyton biomass. In oligotrophic wetlands, traditional measures of water column SRP and TP will substantially underestimate P loading because biotically incorporated P is displaced from the water column to benthic surfaces. Using periphyton TP as a metric of P enrichment is uncomplicated and analogous to pelagic TP assessments in lakes where most P is sequestered in phytoplankton.


Subject(s)
Environmental Monitoring/methods , Phosphorus/analysis , Water Supply , Biomass , Ecosystem , Eukaryota , Florida , Phytoplankton , Soil
4.
Neuroscience ; 120(1): 249-59, 2003.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12849757

ABSTRACT

A significant number of the cholinergic neurons in the basal forebrain of the primate, but not the rodent brain contain the calcium binding protein calbindin-D28k (CB). Previous experiments in our laboratory have demonstrated a substantial age-related loss of CB from the human basal forebrain cholinergic neurons (BFCN). The present study investigated the possible age-related loss of CB from the BFCN in a non-human primate species, the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus). Quantitative analysis of matching sections as well as unbiased stereological determination of neuronal number were used in 16 adult marmosets ranging in age between 2 and 15 years. No significant changes were observed in the number of choline acetyltransferase-positive BFCN when a group of young animals (< or =4 years) was compared with a 6-8-year-old group and a 9-15-year-old group. Similarly, no age-related changes were observed in Nissl-stained magnocellular basal forebrain (putatively cholinergic) neurons. In contrast, the BFCN of the two older groups of animals displayed a significant loss of CB. The age-related loss of CB occurred in all sectors of the BFCN, but was greatest in the anterior sector of this cell group. The CB loss was neurochemically specific since the BFCN in the older groups of animals continued to express other markers such as high and low affinity neurotrophin receptors. The age-related loss of CB from the marmoset BFCN was also regionally selective as CB positive neurons in other structures, such as the cerebral cortex and the striatum displayed no apparent age-related changes. These results indicate that the marmoset BFCN display a significant and selective age-related loss of CB reminiscent of that observed in the human. Therefore, the common marmoset represents an appropriate animal model in which the consequences of BFCN CB loss can be investigated in depth. Loss of CB from the aged BFCN is likely to reduce the capacity of these neurons to buffer intracellular calcium and to leave them vulnerable to insults which can result in increased calcium levels. The vulnerability of the CB-negative BFCN in the aged marmoset to various insults which disturb calcium homeostasis remains to be investigated.


Subject(s)
Aging/metabolism , Cholinergic Fibers/metabolism , Prosencephalon/metabolism , S100 Calcium Binding Protein G/metabolism , Aging/pathology , Animals , Calbindin 1 , Calbindins , Callithrix , Cholinergic Fibers/chemistry , Cholinergic Fibers/pathology , Prosencephalon/chemistry , Prosencephalon/pathology , S100 Calcium Binding Protein G/analysis
5.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 15(2): 294-313, 2003 Feb 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12683359

ABSTRACT

Despite the important role that attending to novel events plays in human behavior, there is limited information about the neuroanatomical underpinnings of this vital activity. This study investigated the relative contributions of the frontal and posterior parietal lobes to the differential processing of novel and target stimuli under an experimental condition in which subjects actively directed attention to novel events. Event-related potentials were recorded from well-matched frontal patients, parietal patients, and non-brain-injured subjects who controlled their viewing duration (by button press) of line drawings that included a frequent, repetitive background stimulus, an infrequent target stimulus, and infrequent, novel visual stimuli. Subjects also responded to target stimuli by pressing a foot pedal. Damage to the frontal cortex resulted in a much greater disruption of response to novel stimuli than to designated targets. Frontal patients exhibited a widely distributed, profound reduction of the novelty P3 response and a marked diminution of the viewing duration of novel events. In contrast, damage to posterior parietal lobes was associated with a substantial reduction of both target P3 and novelty P3 amplitude; however, there was less disruption of the processing of novel than of target stimuli. We conclude that two nodes of the neuroanatomical network for responding to and processing novelty are the prefrontal and posterior parietal regions, which participate in the voluntary allocation of attention to novel events. Injury to this network is indexed by reduced novelty P3 amplitude, which is tightly associated with diminished attention to novel stimuli. The prefrontal cortex may serve as the central node in determining the allocation of attentional resources to novel events, whereas the posterior parietal lobe may provide the neural substrate for the dynamic process of updating one's internal model of the environment to take into account a novel event.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Brain Mapping , Cerebral Infarction/physiopathology , Frontal Lobe/physiology , Nerve Net/physiology , Parietal Lobe/physiology , Aged , Case-Control Studies , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Exploratory Behavior/physiology , Frontal Lobe/physiopathology , Humans , Matched-Pair Analysis , Mental Processes/physiology , Middle Aged , Nerve Net/diagnostic imaging , Nerve Net/physiopathology , Parietal Lobe/physiopathology , Radionuclide Imaging
6.
Neurobiol Aging ; 22(5): 729-36, 2001.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11705632

ABSTRACT

Exaggerated pupillary response to a low concentration of cholinergic antagonists has been suggested as an early marker for Alzheimer's Disease (AD). To examine the anatomic basis of this phenomenon, we determined possible neuropathological changes in the Edinger-Westphal (EW) nucleus, a midbrain neural center with a significant functional role in the control of pupil size. Stereologically determined neuronal numbers within the EW were counted in individuals with pathologically confirmed AD, control cases with no AD-type pathology, and subjects with AD pathology not meeting diagnostic criteria for AD. The EW of AD patients displayed a marked and striking neuronal loss when compared with controls. In contrast, the number of neurons in the somatic portion of the nucleus of the third cranial nerve (NCNIII) remained intact. The EW in brains from clinically normal individuals with evidence of early AD-type pathology also displayed a significant and selective loss of neurons. The magnitude of EW neuronal loss in the latter group was smaller than that observed in AD. These findings suggest that pupillary hypersensitivity in AD may be caused by abnormalities in the EW. Neuronal loss and pathology within the EW in a subpopulation of clinically silent controls with pathologic findings consistent with early-stage AD constitutes a possible explanation for the reported exaggerated pupil response in some normal elderly subjects.


Subject(s)
Alzheimer Disease/pathology , Mesencephalon/pathology , Oculomotor Nerve/pathology , Aged , Amyloid beta-Peptides/analysis , Cell Count , Humans , Neurofibrillary Tangles/pathology , Neurons/chemistry , Neurons/pathology , Phosphorylation , Reflex, Pupillary , tau Proteins/analysis , tau Proteins/metabolism
7.
Neurology ; 56(10): 1377-83, 2001 May 22.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11376191

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Patients with mild to moderate AD often are apathetic and fail to attend to novel aspects of their environment. OBJECTIVE: To investigate the mechanisms underlying these changes by studying the novelty P3 response that measures shifts of attention toward novel events. METHODS: While event-related potentials were recorded, mildly impaired AD patients and matched normal controls (NC) viewed line drawings that included a repetitive background stimulus, an infrequent target stimulus, and infrequent novel stimuli. Subjects controlled how long they viewed each stimulus by pressing a button. This served as a measure of their allocation of attention. They also responded to targets by depressing a foot pedal. Patients did not differ from NC in age, education, estimated IQ, or mood but were judged by informants to be more apathetic. RESULTS: P3 amplitude to novel stimuli was significantly smaller for AD patients than NC. However, P3 amplitude to target stimuli did not differ between groups. For NC, P3 response to novel stimuli was much larger than to background stimuli. In contrast, for patients with AD, there was no difference in P3 response to novel vs background stimuli. Although NC spent more time looking at novel than background stimuli, patients with AD distributed their viewing time evenly. Remarkably, for patients with AD, the amplitude of the novelty P3 response powerfully predicted how long they would spend looking at novel stimuli (R2 = 0.52) and inversely correlated with apathy severity. CONCLUSIONS: The decreased attention to novel events exhibited by patients with AD cannot be explained by a nonspecific reduction in their attentional abilities. The novelty P3 response is markedly diminished in mild AD, at a time when the target P3 response is preserved. The disruption of the novelty P3 response predicts diminished attention to novel stimuli and is associated with the apathy exhibited by patients with AD.


Subject(s)
Alzheimer Disease/physiopathology , Attention/physiology , Aged , Alzheimer Disease/pathology , Cerebral Cortex/pathology , Cerebral Cortex/physiopathology , Disease Progression , Event-Related Potentials, P300/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation , Predictive Value of Tests , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology
8.
Psychophysiology ; 37(6): 737-47, 2000 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11117454

ABSTRACT

This study investigated the functional significance of the N2 response to novel stimuli. In one condition, background, target, and deviant stimuli were simple geometric figures. In a second condition, all stimulus types were unfamiliar/unusual figures. In a third condition, background and target stimuli were unusual figures and deviant stimuli were simple shapes. Unusual figures, whether they were deviant, target, or background stimuli, evoked larger N2 responses than their simple, familiar counterparts. N2 elicited by an unusual background stimulus was larger than that evoked by simple, deviant stimuli, a pattern opposite that exhibited by the subsequent P3. Deviance from immediate context had limited influence over N2 amplitude. The results suggest that novelty N2 and novelty P3 reflect the processing of different aspects of "novel" visual stimuli. The novelty P3 is particularly sensitive to deviation from immediate context. In contrast, the novelty N2 is sensitive to deviation from long-term context that renders a stimulus unfamiliar and difficult to encode.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Discrimination Learning/physiology , Electroencephalography , Mental Recall/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Adult , Brain Mapping , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Evoked Potentials, Visual/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Problem Solving/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology
9.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 12(3): 393-406, 2000 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10931766

ABSTRACT

This study investigated the role of stimulus deviance in determining electrophysiologic and behavioral responses to "novelty." Stimulus deviance was defined in terms of differences either from the immediately preceding context or from long-term experience. Subjects participated in a visual event-related potential (ERP) experiment, in which they controlled the duration of stimulus viewing with a button press, which served as a measure of exploratory behavior. Each of the three experimental conditions included a frequent repetitive background stimulus and infrequent stimuli that deviated from the background stimulus. In one condition, both background and deviant stimuli were simple, easily recognizable geometric figures. In another condition, both background and deviant stimuli were unusual/unfamiliar figures, and in a third condition, the background stimulus was a highly unusual figure, and the deviant stimuli were simple, geometric shapes. Deviant stimuli elicited larger N2-P3 amplitudes and longer viewing durations than the repetitive background stimulus, even when the deviant stimuli were simple, familiar shapes and the background stimulus was a highly unusual figure. Compared to simple, familiar deviant stimuli, unusual deviant stimuli elicited larger N2-P3 amplitudes and longer viewing times. Within subjects, the deviant stimuli that evoked the largest N2-P3 responses also elicited the longest viewing durations. We conclude that deviance from both immediate context and long-term prior experience contribute to the response to novelty, with the combination generating the largest N2-P3 amplitude and the most sustained attention. The amplitude of the N2-P3 may reflect how much "uncertainty" is evoked by a novel visual stimulus and signal the need for further exploration and cognitive processing.


Subject(s)
Evoked Potentials, Visual/physiology , Exploratory Behavior/physiology , Form Perception/physiology , Adult , Event-Related Potentials, P300/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation
10.
Brain ; 123 ( Pt 5): 927-39, 2000 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10775538

ABSTRACT

The physiological basis for the striking decrease of attention to novel events following frontal lobe injury is poorly understood. In this study, event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded from patients with frontal lobe damage and matched subjects, who controlled the duration of viewing of background, novel and target stimuli. Frontal lobe patients did not differ from normal controls in terms of age, education, estimated IQ or mood. However, they were judged to be more apathetic as measured by self-report and informants' ratings. Patients with frontal lobe damage exhibited markedly reduced amplitude of the novelty P3 response and the duration of viewing of novel stimuli. In contrast, injury to the frontal lobes had a limited impact on P3 amplitude and behavioural responses (viewing duration and reaction time) to target stimuli. A strong correlation was found between measures of apathy and both attenuated P3 amplitude and viewing duration in response to novel but not target stimuli. Differences in amplitude of the novelty P3 response explained a large portion of the variance associated with duration of viewing of novel stimuli. After controlling for the influence of P3 amplitude, there was no association between frontal lobe injury and reduced viewing of novel stimuli. The results of this study suggest that frontal lobe damage leads to diminished visual attention to novel events through its disruption of neural processes underlying the novelty P3 response. These processes appear to regulate the allocation of attentional resources and early exploratory behaviours, and are not limited to immediate orienting responses. Damage to the frontal lobes may prevent the generation of a signal which indicates that a novel event in the environment requires additional attention due to its potential behavioural significance. The disruption of these processes is likely to contribute to the apathy observed in patients after injury to the frontal lobes.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Brain Mapping , Brain/physiology , Cerebral Infarction/psychology , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology , Adult , Affect , Aged , Brain/physiopathology , Cerebral Infarction/pathology , Cerebral Infarction/physiopathology , Female , Form Perception , Humans , Intelligence , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged , Photic Stimulation , Prefrontal Cortex/pathology , Prefrontal Cortex/physiopathology , Reference Values , Tomography, X-Ray Computed
11.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry ; 68(1): 18-24, 2000 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10601395

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To investigate whether frontal lobe damage in humans disrupts the natural tendency to preferentially attend to novel visual events in the environment. METHODS: Nine patients with chronic infarctions in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and 23 matched normal controls participated in a study in which subjects viewed repetitive background stimuli, infrequent target stimuli, and novel visual stimuli (for example, fragmented or "impossible" objects). Subjects controlled viewing duration by a button press that led to the onset of the next stimulus. They also responded to targets by pressing a foot pedal. The amount of time spent looking at the different kinds of stimuli, and the target detection accuracy and speed served as dependent variables. RESULTS: Overall, normal controls spent significantly more time than frontal lobe patients looking at novel stimuli. Analysis of responses across blocks showed that initially frontal lobe patients behaved like normal controls by directing more attention to novel than background stimuli. However, they quickly began to distribute their viewing time evenly between novel and background stimuli, a pattern that was strikingly different from normal controls. By contrast, there were no differences between frontal lobe patients and normal controls for viewing duration devoted to background and target stimuli, target detection accuracy, or reaction time to targets. Frontal lobe patients did not differ from normal controls in terms of age, education, estimated IQ, or mood, but were more apathetic as measured by self report and informants' judgments. Attenuated responses to novel stimuli significantly correlated with degree of apathy. CONCLUSIONS: This study demonstrates that DLPFC injury selectively impairs the natural tendency to seek stimulation from novel and unusual stimuli. These data provide the first quantitative behavioural demonstration that the human frontal lobes play a critical part in directing and sustaining attention to novel events. The impairment of novelty seeking behaviour may contribute to the characteristic apathy found in patients with frontal lobe injury.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Brain Injuries/physiopathology , Frontal Lobe/physiopathology , Aged , Brain Injuries/psychology , Brain Mapping , Frontal Lobe/diagnostic imaging , Frontal Lobe/pathology , Humans , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Radiography , Reaction Time/physiology
12.
Acta Neuropathol ; 97(6): 557-64, 1999 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10378374

ABSTRACT

Patients who suffer from Alzheimer's disease (AD) and a sub-population of community-dwelling elders show an exaggerated pupillary reaction to dilute tropicamide, a cholinergic antagonist. This finding may serve as an early diagnostic marker of AD. Here we report a likely pathological basis for this hypersensitive pupillary response. Our observations indicate that the Edinger-Westphal nucleus (EW), a known center for the control of pupillary function, is a selective target of Alzheimer pathology early in the course of the disease. In all AD cases examined, the EW contained plaques and tangles. In contrast, the adjacent somatic portion of the oculomotor complex was virtually spared of pathology. Early pathology in the EW is likely to initiate a cascade of events that may give rise to pupillary hypersensitivity.


Subject(s)
Alzheimer Disease/pathology , Oculomotor Nerve/pathology , Pupil/physiology , Alzheimer Disease/physiopathology , Brain/pathology , Brain/physiopathology , Humans , Immunohistochemistry , Oculomotor Nerve/physiopathology , Reflex, Pupillary/physiology
13.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10082334

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To better understand apathy and disengagement in patients with Alzheimer's disease (AD), the authors investigated possible behavioral mechanisms underlying diminished novelty-seeking activity in patients with probable AD. BACKGROUND: Apathy and disengagement have been shown to be the most common behavioral changes associated with AD. METHOD: Patients and age-matched normal controls had their eye movements recorded while pairs of line drawings pitting an incongruous figure against a congruous figure were shown on a screen for 12 seconds. Characteristics of a subset of AD patients who were indifferent to novel visual stimuli as measured by exploratory eye movements were compared to those of a subset of AD patients who were attracted to novel stimuli to a degree similar to that of normal controls. RESULTS: The indifferent patients were judged by informants, who completed a personality questionnaire, to exhibit a greater degree of apathy. The two AD groups did not differ in overall dementia severity or performance on a Saccade-to-Target Task that required shifts of attention and gaze. In a separate task, the indifferent patients were able to accurately identify the more novel stimuli in 97.5% of trials. Normal control subjects exhibited a strong bias toward processing novel stimuli, directing a higher proportion of their first fixations and dwell time to the incongruous stimuli whether the analysis was run for 3, 6, or 12 seconds of viewing. Indifferent patients did not direct their initial fixation toward novel stimuli and distributed their looking time evenly between incongruous and congruous stimuli throughout all measured intervals. CONCLUSIONS: The results suggest that the indifference to novelty observed in some patients with probable AD cannot simply be attributed to global cognitive decline, more elementary attentional deficits, more rapid habituation of response to novel stimuli, or an inability to discriminate upon demand between stimuli of varying degrees of novelty. It is more likely that their behavior reflects a disruption, by AD pathology, of neural systems that modulate behavioral engagement and maintain attentional bias toward novel events in the environment.


Subject(s)
Alzheimer Disease/psychology , Exploratory Behavior , Aged , Alzheimer Disease/diagnosis , Attention/physiology , Exploratory Behavior/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Personality Inventory , Reaction Time , Saccades/physiology , Severity of Illness Index
15.
Neuroreport ; 9(5): 787-91, 1998 Mar 30.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9579666

ABSTRACT

This study examined the relationship between orienting responses to novel events and subsequent exploratory behavior. The N2-P3 electrophysiologic component of the orienting response was found to be larger for novel than repetitive background stimuli. Across subjects, the amplitude of this N2-P3 response in frontal regions strongly predicted the proportional increase in the duration of viewing directed toward novel compared to background stimuli. Within subjects, larger N2-P3 amplitudes in response to novel stimuli were associated with longer viewing durations on those stimuli. These results suggest that the N2-P3 component of the orienting response reflects the activity of a neural system involving frontal networks that dynamically regulates the subsequent allocation of attentional resources to novel stimuli.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Frontal Lobe/physiology , Adult , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Orientation/physiology , Photic Stimulation , Time Factors
16.
Neuroreport ; 8(4): 901-5, 1997 Mar 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9141061

ABSTRACT

We investigated whether adolescents and adults with the developmental social-emotional processing disorder (SEPD) exhibit deficits in visual attention, as measured by eye movements, when compared with dyslexic and normal control subjects. On the antisaccade task, subjects with SEPD made more errors than either control group and were the only group to show a decrease in performance accuracy compared with prosaccade. This deficit in inhibiting reflexive shifts of attention and gaze suggests that individuals with SEPD have dysfunction of the prefrontal component of the right hemisphere dominant network for spatially directed attention.


Subject(s)
Learning Disabilities/physiopathology , Saccades/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Attention , Dyslexia/physiopathology , Emotions , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Reaction Time , Syndrome , Time
17.
Ann Neurol ; 39(2): 174-9, 1996 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8967748

ABSTRACT

We employed positron emission tomography to examine the functional anatomy of the exploratory-motor aspect of spatial attention. Subjects moved their right hand under nonexploratory (control) versus exploratory (active) conditions. Movement architecture and eye movements were matched across conditions. Statistical parametric maps of the exploratory (active) minus nonexploratory (control) tasks in subjects using their right hand in the right hemispace demonstrated activation in right cingulate, premotor and posterior parietal areas. The net activation of multiple right hemisphere cortical areas ipsilateral to the active limb and hemispace strongly supports the predicted, distributed network anatomy and is consistent with possible right hemispheric dominance for exploratory attentional movements.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Exploratory Behavior/physiology , Functional Laterality/physiology , Adult , Attention/physiology , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Brain Mapping , Cerebrovascular Circulation , Eye Movements , Hand/physiology , Humans , Male , Motor Cortex/blood supply , Movement/physiology , Somatosensory Cortex/blood supply , Space Perception/physiology , Tomography, Emission-Computed
18.
Science ; 267(5204): 1580-1, 1995 Mar 17.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17808112
19.
Science ; 266(5187): 1051-4, 1994 Nov 11.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7973660

ABSTRACT

Currently Alzheimer's disease, which affects more than 20 million people worldwide, can only be definitely diagnosed by histological examination of brain tissue obtained at autopsy or biopsy. There is a great need for an early, noninvasive, sensitive, and easily administered diagnostic test of Alzheimer's disease. Here it is reported that patients diagnosed with probable Alzheimer's disease by standard clinical criteria exhibited a marked hypersensitivity in their pupil dilation response to a cholinergic antagonist, tropicamide, placed in their eyes. It was possible to distinguish 18 of 19 individuals (95%) either clinically diagnosed with Alzheimer's disease or classified as suspect Alzheimer's individuals by neuropsychological screening from 30 of 32 normal elderly controls (94%).


Subject(s)
Alzheimer Disease/diagnosis , Pupil/drug effects , Tropicamide , Aged , Alzheimer Disease/physiopathology , Dementia/physiopathology , Female , Humans , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Reproducibility of Results , Sensitivity and Specificity , Tropicamide/pharmacology
20.
Arch Neurol ; 51(7): 682-8, 1994 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8018041

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To investigate changes in spatially directed attention in patients with a diagnosis of probable Alzheimer's disease (AD). BACKGROUND: Impaired attention in patients with probable AD has not been the subject of extensive research. Yet recent reports suggest that attentional deficits may be an important early feature of the disease in a subset of patients. SETTING: University hospital center studying dementia and aging. SUBJECTS: Ten mild to moderately impaired patients diagnosed as having probable AD, by National Institute of Neurologic and Communicative Diseases and Stroke criteria, and 11 healthy age- and education-matched controls. MEASURES: Eye movements were recorded as subjects participated in two experiments designed to measure spatially directed attention. Subjects were instructed to (1) attend to and fixate a target appearing randomly to the right or left of a central marker and (2) direct attention to and fixate a target appearing randomly in one of four peripheral locations. RESULTS: Patients with probable AD exhibited fewer accurate trials and longer saccade latencies in both tasks. As a group, patients performed worse in the second task that placed increased demands on attention. However, the performance of patients in this second experiment varied. Four patients performed significantly worse than all other patients, while three patients performed as well as controls. Errors in the second task were reviewed to identify specific types of attentional deficits. Six empirically derived error patterns were classified into one of two major categories: perseveration and impersistence. Seven of 10 patients made greater than 50% errors of perseveration, and three of 10 made greater than 50% errors of impersistence. CONCLUSIONS: Impairment of attention may be an early feature of AD and a prominent clinical characteristic of some patients. The differences observed in error types made by patients may reflect the varied distribution of neuropathologic changes affecting structures that mediate aspects of attention. The architecture of eye movements can be used as a physiologic measure that should provide useful information for the diagnosis and clinicopathologic subtyping of patients with AD.


Subject(s)
Alzheimer Disease/physiopathology , Alzheimer Disease/psychology , Attention , Eye Movements , Aged , Female , Fixation, Ocular , Humans , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Spatial Behavior
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...