Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 29
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Dev Cogn Neurosci ; 37: 100614, 2019 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30777677

ABSTRACT

Visuospatial abilities such as contrast sensitivity and Vernier acuity improve until late in childhood, but the neural mechanisms supporting these changes are poorly understood. We tested to which extent this development might reflect improved spatial sensitivity of neuronal populations in visual cortex. To do this, we measured BOLD-responses in areas V1-V4 and V3a, whilst 6- to 12-year-old children and adults watched large-field wedge and ring stimuli in the MRI scanner, and then fitted population receptive field (pRF) tuning functions to these data (Dumoulin and Wandell, 2008). Cortical magnification and pRF tuning width changed with eccentricity at all ages, as expected. However, there were no significant age differences in pRF size, shape, cortical magnification, or map consistency in any visual region. These findings thus strongly suggest that spatial vision in late childhood is not substantially limited by the spatial tuning of neuronal populations in early visual cortex. Instead, improvements in performance may reflect more efficient read-out of spatial information in early visual regions by higher-level processing stages, or prolonged tuning to more complex visual properties such as orientation. Importantly, this in-depth characterisation of the pRF tuning profiles across childhood, paves the way for in-vivo-testing of atypical visual cortex development and plasticity.


Subject(s)
Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Visual Cortex/physiology , Child , Female , Humans , Male
2.
Open Neuroimag J ; 10: 1-19, 2016.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27014392

ABSTRACT

Deafness results in greater reliance on the remaining senses. It is unknown whether the cortical architecture of the intact senses is optimized to compensate for lost input. Here we performed widefield population receptive field (pRF) mapping of primary visual cortex (V1) with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in hearing and congenitally deaf participants, all of whom had learnt sign language after the age of 10 years. We found larger pRFs encoding the peripheral visual field of deaf compared to hearing participants. This was likely driven by larger facilitatory center zones of the pRF profile concentrated in the near and far periphery in the deaf group. pRF density was comparable between groups, indicating pRFs overlapped more in the deaf group. This could suggest that a coarse coding strategy underlies enhanced peripheral visual skills in deaf people. Cortical thickness was also decreased in V1 in the deaf group. These findings suggest deafness causes structural and functional plasticity at the earliest stages of visual cortex.

3.
Neuroimage ; 82: 517-30, 2013 Nov 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23770406

ABSTRACT

In macaque monkeys, V6A is a visuomotor area located in the anterior bank of the POs, dorsal and anterior to retinotopically-organized extrastriate area V6 (Galletti et al., 1996). Unlike V6, V6A represents both contra- and ipsilateral visual fields and is broadly retinotopically organized (Galletti et al., 1999b). The contralateral lower visual field is over-represented in V6A. The central 20°-30° of the visual field is mainly represented dorsally (V6Ad) and the periphery ventrally (V6Av), at the border with V6. Both sectors of area V6A contain arm movement-related cells, active during spatially-directed reaching movements (Gamberini et al., 2011). In humans, we previously mapped the retinotopic organization of area V6 (Pitzalis et al., 2006). Here, using phase-encoded fMRI, cortical surface-based analysis and wide-field retinotopic mapping, we define a new cortical region that borders V6 anteriorly and shows a clear over-representation of the contralateral lower visual field and the periphery. As with macaque V6A, the eccentricity increases moving ventrally within the area. The new region contains a non-mirror-image representation of the visual field. Functional mapping reveals that, as in macaque V6A, the new region, but not the nearby area V6, responds during finger pointing and reaching movements. Based on similarity in position, retinotopic properties, functional organization and relationship with the neighboring extrastriate visual areas, we propose that the new cortical region is the human homologue of macaque area V6A.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping , Visual Cortex/anatomy & histology , Adult , Animals , Female , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Macaca , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Photic Stimulation , Visual Pathways/anatomy & histology
4.
Cereb Cortex ; 20(2): 411-24, 2010 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19502476

ABSTRACT

Cortical-surface-based functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging mapping techniques and wide-field retinotopic stimulation were used to verify the presence of pattern motion sensitivity in human area V6. Area V6 is highly selective for coherently moving fields of dots, both at individual and group levels and even with a visual stimulus of standard size. This stimulus is a functional localizer for V6. The wide retinotopic stimuli used here also revealed a retinotopic map in the middle temporal cortex (area MT/V5) surrounded by several polar-angle maps that resemble the mosaic of small areas found around macaque MT/V5. Our results suggest that the MT complex (MT+) may be specialized for the analysis of motion signals, whereas area V6 may be more involved in distinguishing object and self-motion.


Subject(s)
Motion Perception/physiology , Occipital Lobe/physiology , Parietal Lobe/physiology , Visual Cortex/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Brain Mapping , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Female , Functional Laterality/physiology , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Occipital Lobe/anatomy & histology , Parietal Lobe/anatomy & histology , Photic Stimulation , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Retina/physiology , Visual Cortex/anatomy & histology , Visual Fields/physiology , Visual Pathways/anatomy & histology , Visual Pathways/physiology , Young Adult
5.
Neuroimage ; 35(4): 1562-77, 2007 May 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17376706

ABSTRACT

A recent study from our laboratory demonstrated that parietal cortex contains a map of visual space related to saccades and spatial attention and identified this area as the likely human homologue of the lateral intraparietal (LIP). A human homologue for the parietal reach region (PRR), thought to preferentially encode planned hand movements, has also been recently proposed. Both of these areas, originally identified in the macaque monkey, have been shown to encode space with eye-centered coordinates. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) of humans was used to test the hypothesis that the putative human PRR contains a retinotopic map recruited by finger pointing but not saccades and to test more generally for differences in the visuospatial maps recruited by pointing and saccades. We identified multiple maps in both posterior parietal cortex and superior frontal cortex recruited for eye and hand movements, including maps not observed in previous mapping studies. Pointing and saccade maps were generally consistent within single subjects. We have developed new group analysis methods for phase-encoded data, which revealed subtle differences between pointing and saccades, including hemispheric asymmetries, but we did not find evidence of pointing-specific maps of visual space.


Subject(s)
Frontal Lobe/physiology , Movement/physiology , Parietal Lobe/physiology , Saccades/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adult , Algorithms , Brain Mapping , Female , Fingers/physiology , Fourier Analysis , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Photic Stimulation
6.
Science ; 294(5545): 1350-4, 2001 Nov 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11701930

ABSTRACT

The internal organization of a higher level visual area in the human parietal cortex was mapped. Functional magnetic resonance images were acquired while the polar angle of a peripheral target for a delayed saccade was gradually changed. A region in the superior parietal cortex showed robust retinotopic mapping of the remembered target angle. The map reversed when the direction of rotation of the remembered targets was reversed and persisted unchanged when study participants detected rare target reappearances while maintaining fixation, or when the eccentricity of successive remembered targets was unpredictable. This region may correspond to the lateral intraparietal area in macaque monkeys.


Subject(s)
Parietal Lobe/physiology , Saccades/physiology , Visual Pathways/physiology , Visual Perception , Brain Mapping , Echo-Planar Imaging , Fixation, Ocular , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Memory/physiology , Parietal Lobe/anatomy & histology , Visual Cortex/physiology
7.
Vision Res ; 41(10-11): 1437-57, 2001.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11322985

ABSTRACT

This study investigated the cortical mechanisms of visual-spatial attention in a task where subjects discriminated patterned targets in one visual field at a time. Functional magnetic imaging (fMRI) was used to localize attention-related changes in neural activity within specific retinotopic visual areas, while recordings of event-related brain potentials (ERPs) traced the time course of these changes. The earliest ERP components enhanced by attention occurred in the time range 70-130 ms post-stimulus onset, and their neural generators were estimated to lie in the dorsal and ventral extrastriate visual cortex. The anatomical areas activated by attention corresponded closely to those showing increased neural activity during passive visual stimulation. Enhanced neural activity was also observed in the primary visual cortex (area V1) with fMRI, but ERP recordings indicated that the initial sensory response at 50-90 ms that was localized to V1 was not modulated by attention. Modeling of ERP sources over an extended time range showed that attended stimuli elicited a long-latency (160-260 ms) negativity that was attributed to the dipolar source in area V1. This finding is in line with hypotheses that V1 activity may be modulated by delayed, reentrant feedback from higher visual areas.


Subject(s)
Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Visual Cortex/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Algorithms , Electrophysiology , Evoked Potentials, Visual , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Time Factors
8.
Brain ; 122 ( Pt 7): 1317-25, 1999 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10388797

ABSTRACT

We examined the brain areas involved in discourse processing by using functional MRI in 10 individuals as they read paragraphs, with or without a title, word by word for comprehension. Functional data were collected from 20 adjacent 5 mm axial slices. Discourse processing was associated with activation in inferior frontal and temporal regions of both cerebral hemispheres in the titled and untitled conditions. Moreover, there was substantially more right hemisphere activation for untitled than for the titled paragraphs. More specifically we found: (i) greater activation in the inferior temporal sulcus of both hemispheres for untitled than titled paragraphs; (ii) greater average volume of activation in response to untitled than titled paragraphs in the middle temporal sulcus of the right hemisphere and the reverse pattern in the left middle temporal sulcus. Consistent with previous studies of individuals with right hemisphere damage, we suggest that the right middle temporal regions may be especially important for integrative processes needed to achieve global coherence during discourse processing.


Subject(s)
Dominance, Cerebral/physiology , Reading , Semantics , Adult , Brain/physiology , Evoked Potentials/physiology , Female , Frontal Lobe/physiology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Mental Processes/physiology , Middle Aged , Temporal Lobe/physiology
9.
Nat Neurosci ; 2(4): 364-9, 1999 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10204544

ABSTRACT

We investigated the cortical mechanisms of visual-spatial attention while subjects discriminated patterned targets within distractor arrays. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) was used to map the boundaries of retinotopic visual areas and to localize attention-related changes in neural activity within several of those areas, including primary visual (striate) cortex. Event-related potentials (ERPs) and modeling of their neural sources, however, indicated that the initial sensory input to striate cortex at 50-55 milliseconds after the stimulus was not modulated by attention. The earliest facilitation of attended signals was observed in extrastriate visual areas, at 70-75 milliseconds. We hypothesize that the striate cortex modulation found with fMRI may represent a delayed, re-entrant feedback from higher visual areas or a sustained biasing of striate cortical neurons during attention. ERP recordings provide critical temporal information for analyzing the functional neuroanatomy of visual attention.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Visual Cortex/physiology , Adult , Brain Mapping , Cerebrovascular Circulation , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Evoked Potentials, Visual , Feedback , Female , Geniculate Bodies/physiology , Humans , Models, Neurological , Parietal Lobe/physiology , Visual Pathways/physiology
10.
Neuroimage ; 9(2): 179-94, 1999 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9931268

ABSTRACT

Several properties of the cerebral cortex, including its columnar and laminar organization, as well as the topographic organization of cortical areas, can only be properly understood in the context of the intrinsic two-dimensional structure of the cortical surface. In order to study such cortical properties in humans, it is necessary to obtain an accurate and explicit representation of the cortical surface in individual subjects. Here we describe a set of automated procedures for obtaining accurate reconstructions of the cortical surface, which have been applied to data from more than 100 subjects, requiring little or no manual intervention. Automated routines for unfolding and flattening the cortical surface are described in a companion paper. These procedures allow for the routine use of cortical surface-based analysis and visualization methods in functional brain imaging.


Subject(s)
Cerebral Cortex/anatomy & histology , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted/instrumentation , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/instrumentation , Brain Mapping/instrumentation , Humans , Reference Values , Software
11.
Neuroimage ; 9(2): 195-207, 1999 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9931269

ABSTRACT

The surface of the human cerebral cortex is a highly folded sheet with the majority of its surface area buried within folds. As such, it is a difficult domain for computational as well as visualization purposes. We have therefore designed a set of procedures for modifying the representation of the cortical surface to (i) inflate it so that activity buried inside sulci may be visualized, (ii) cut and flatten an entire hemisphere, and (iii) transform a hemisphere into a simple parameterizable surface such as a sphere for the purpose of establishing a surface-based coordinate system.


Subject(s)
Cerebral Cortex/anatomy & histology , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted/instrumentation , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/instrumentation , Artifacts , Brain Mapping/instrumentation , Dominance, Cerebral/physiology , Humans , Software
12.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 7(1): 29-37, 1999.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9882088

ABSTRACT

Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) was used to identify a small area in the human posterior fusiform gyrus that responds selectively to faces (PF). In the same subjects, phase-encoded rotating and expanding checkerboards were used with fMRI to identify the retinotopic visual areas V1, V2, V3, V3A, VP and V4v. PF was found to lie anterior to area V4v, with a small gap present between them. Further recordings in some of the same subjects used moving low-contrast rings to identify the visual motion area MT. PF was found to lie ventral to MT. In addition, preliminary evidence was found using fMRI for a small area that responded to inanimate objects but not to faces in the collateral sulcus medial to PF. The retinotopic visual areas and MT responded equally to faces, control randomized stimuli, and objects. Weakly face-selective responses were also found in ventrolateral occipitotemporal cortex anterior to V4v, as well as in the middle temporal gyrus anterior to MT. We conclude that the fusiform face area in humans lies in non-retinotopic visual association cortex of the ventral form-processing stream, in an area that may be roughly homologous in location to area TF or CITv in monkeys.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Face , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Retina/physiology , Temporal Lobe/physiology , Visual Pathways/physiology , Adult , Cerebral Cortex/blood supply , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Male , Regional Blood Flow , Visual Pathways/blood supply
13.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 8(4): 272-84, 1999.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10619420

ABSTRACT

The neurons of the human cerebral cortex are arranged in a highly folded sheet, with the majority of the cortical surface area buried in folds. Cortical maps are typically arranged with a topography oriented parallel to the cortical surface. Despite this unambiguous sheetlike geometry, the most commonly used coordinate systems for localizing cortical features are based on 3-D stereotaxic coordinates rather than on position relative to the 2-D cortical sheet. In order to address the need for a more natural surface-based coordinate system for the cortex, we have developed a means for generating an average folding pattern across a large number of individual subjects as a function on the unit sphere and of nonrigidly aligning each individual with the average. This establishes a spherical surface-based coordinate system that is adapted to the folding pattern of each individual subject, allowing for much higher localization accuracy of structural and functional features of the human brain.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping , Cerebral Cortex/anatomy & histology , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Genetic Variation , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Models, Anatomic , Models, Neurological
14.
J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform ; 25(6): 1834-54, 1999 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10641318

ABSTRACT

This study investigates how mechanisms for amplifying 2-D motion contrast influence the assignment of 3-D depth values. The authors found that the direction of movement of a random-dot conveyor belt strongly inclined observers to report that the front surface of a superimposed, transparent, rotating, random-dot sphere moved in a direction opposite to the belt. This motion-contrast effect was direction selective and demonstrated substantial spatial integration. Varying the stereo depth of the belt did not compromise the main effect, precluding a mechanical interpretation (sphere rolling on belt). Varying the speed of the surfaces of the sphere also did not greatly affect the interpretation of rotation direction. These results suggest that 2-D center-surround interactions influence 3-D depth assignment by differentially modulating the strength of response to the moving surfaces of an object (their prominence) without affecting featural specificity.


Subject(s)
Attention , Depth Perception , Motion Perception , Optical Illusions , Orientation , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Adult , Contrast Sensitivity , Discrimination Learning , Female , Humans , Male , Psychophysics
15.
Cereb Cortex ; 8(4): 362-71, 1998 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9651131

ABSTRACT

Extracellular recordings obtained from the extrastriate cortex of the California ground squirrel, a diurnal sciurid, show that large receptive fields and a strong direction selectivity are present in the middle lateral area (ML) and the lateral area (L), located laterally to V2 and V3. Direction selectivity was tested by presenting stimuli of varying dimensions, shapes and speeds at different locations in the visual field. Most cells in ML and L (84%) were direction selective, with a preference for fast speeds, indicating that these areas share a role in motion processing. Areas ML and L may be homologous to area MT or may represent a case of homoplasia. A directional anisotropy for motion towards the vertical meridian was found in ML and L cells, suggesting that these areas may be involved in detecting predators and other moving objects coming from the periphery, rather than in processing flow fields caused by forward locomotion, for which a centrifugal bias might be expected.


Subject(s)
Motion Perception/physiology , Sciuridae/physiology , Visual Pathways/physiology , Animals , Anisotropy , Female , Male , Photic Stimulation/methods , Visual Fields/physiology
16.
Curr Opin Neurobiol ; 8(2): 188-94, 1998 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9635201

ABSTRACT

Studies using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to map cortical areas in humans have revealed many similarities with recent cortical mapping studies from nonhuman primates as well as some striking differences. Improved methods for analyzing, displaying and averaging fMRI data on an unfolded cortical surface atlas are poised to improve the integration of information across burgeoning numbers of imaging studies. By combining fMRI with electrical and passive magnetic imaging modalities, the millisecond-to-millisecond sequence of activation of different cortical regions elicited by an event can be imaged, provided the regions are sufficiently far apart.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping/methods , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Primates/physiology , Animals , Data Display , Diagnostic Imaging/methods , Humans , Surface Properties
17.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 95(3): 811-7, 1998 Feb 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9448245

ABSTRACT

Human area V1 offers an excellent opportunity to study, using functional MRI, a range of properties in a specific cortical visual area, whose borders are defined objectively and convergently by retinotopic criteria. The retinotopy in V1 (also known as primary visual cortex, striate cortex, or Brodmann's area 17) was defined in each subject by using both stationary and phase-encoded polar coordinate stimuli. Data from V1 and neighboring retinotopic areas were displayed on flattened cortical maps. In additional tests we revealed the paired cortical representations of the monocular "blind spot." We also activated area V1 preferentially (relative to other extrastriate areas) by presenting radial gratings alternating between 6% and 100% contrast. Finally, we showed evidence for orientation selectivity in V1 by measuring transient functional MRI increases produced at the change in response to gratings of differing orientations. By systematically varying the orientations presented, we were able to measure the bandwidth of the orientation "transients" (45 degrees).


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Visual Cortex/anatomy & histology , Visual Cortex/physiology , Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Humans , Optic Disk/physiology , Orientation/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology
18.
J Neurosci ; 17(18): 7060-78, 1997 Sep 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9278542

ABSTRACT

Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and cortical unfolding techniques, we analyzed the retinotopy, motion sensitivity, and functional organization of human area V3A. These data were compared with data from additional human cortical visual areas, including V1, V2, V3/VP, V4v, and MT (V5). Human V3A has a retinotopy that is similar to that reported previously in macaque: (1) it has a distinctive, continuous map of the contralateral hemifield immediately anterior to area V3, including a unique retinotopic representation of the upper visual field in superior occipital cortex; (2) in some cases the V3A foveal representation is displaced from and superior to the confluent foveal representations of V1, V2, V3, and VP; and (3) inferred receptive fields are significantly larger in human V3A, compared with those in more posterior areas such as V1. However, in other aspects human V3A appears quite different from its macaque counterpart: human V3A is relatively motion-selective, whereas human V3 is less so. In macaque, the situation is qualitatively reversed: V3 is reported to be prominently motion-selective, whereas V3A is less so. As in human and macaque MT, the contrast sensitivity appears quite high in human areas V3 and V3A.


Subject(s)
Visual Cortex/physiology , Anatomy, Cross-Sectional , Animals , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Macaca , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Motion , Photic Stimulation , Visual Cortex/anatomy & histology , Visual Perception/physiology
19.
Nature ; 388(6638): 175-9, 1997 Jul 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9217157

ABSTRACT

Edges are important in the interpretation of the retinal image. Although luminance edges have been studied extensively, much less is known about how or where the primate visual system detects boundaries defined by differences in surface properties such as texture, motion or binocular disparity. Here we use functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to localize human visual cortical activity related to the processing of one such higher-order edge type: motion boundaries. We describe a robust fMRI signal that is selective for motion segmentation. This boundary-specific signal is present, and retinotopically organized, within early visual areas, beginning in the primary visual cortex (area V1). Surprisingly, it is largely absent from the motion-selective area MT/V5 and far extrastriate visual areas. Changes in the surface velocity defining the motion boundaries affect the strength of the fMRI signal. In parallel psychophysical experiments, the perceptual salience of the boundaries shows a similar dependence on surface velocity. These results demonstrate that information for segmenting scenes by relative motion is represented as early as V1.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping , Motion Perception/physiology , Visual Cortex/physiology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods
20.
Trends Neurosci ; 19(11): 481-9, 1996 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8931274

ABSTRACT

Recent developments in imaging and histology have greatly clarified our understanding of the nature and organization of human visual cortex. More than ten human cortical visual areas can now be differentiated, compared with the approximately 30 areas described in macaque monkeys. Most human areas and columns described so far appear quite similar to those in macaque but distinctive species differences also exist. Imaging studies suggest two general information-processing streams (parietal and temporal) in human visual cortex, as proposed in macaque. Several human areas are both motion- and direction-selective, and a progression of motion-processing steps can be-inferred from the imaging data. Human visual areas for recognizing form are less well defined but the evidence again suggests a progression of information-processing steps and areas, beginning posterior to the human middle temporal area (or V5), and extending inferiorly then anteriorly. This is consistent with findings from macaque, and with human clinical reports.


Subject(s)
Visual Cortex/anatomy & histology , Animals , Brain Mapping , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...