Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 11 de 11
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Cortex ; 167: 235-246, 2023 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37579642

ABSTRACT

Performing a secondary task while driving causes a decline in driving performance. This phenomenon, called dual-task interference, can have lethal consequences. Previous fMRI studies have looked at the changes in the average brain activity to uncover the neural correlates of dual-task interference. From these results, it is unclear whether the overall modulations in brain activity result from general effects such as task difficulty, attentional modulations, and mental effort or whether it is caused by a change in the responses specific to each condition due to dual-task interference. To overcome this limitation, here, we used multi-voxel pattern analysis (MVPA) to interrogate the change in the information content in multiple brain regions during dual-task interference in simulated driving. Participants performed a lane-change task in a simulated driving environment, along with a tone discrimination task with either short or long onset time difference (Stimulus Onset Asynchrony, SOA) between the two tasks. Behavioral results indicated a robust dual-task effect on lane-change reaction time (RT). MVPA revealed regions that carry information about the driving lane-change direction (shift right/shift left), including the superior parietal lobe (SPL), visual, and motor regions. Comparison of decoding accuracies across SOA conditions in the SPL region revealed lower accuracy in the short compared to the long SOA condition. This change in accuracy was not observed in the visual and motor regions. These findings suggest that the dual-task interference in driving may be related to the disturbance of information processing in the SPL region.

2.
Neuropsychology ; 35(5): 529-539, 2021 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29658723

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: The semantic fluency task is widely used in both clinical and research settings to assess both the integrity of the semantic store and the effectiveness of the search through it. Our aim was to investigate whether nondemented Parkinson's disease (PD) patients show an impairment in the strategic exploration of the semantic store and whether the tested semantic category has an impact on multiple measures of performance. METHOD: We compared 74 nondemented PD patients with 254 healthy subjects in a semantic fluency test using relatively small (fruits) and large (animals) semantic categories. Number of words produced, number of explored semantic subcategories, and degree of order in the produced sequences were computed as dependent variables. RESULTS: PD patients produced fewer words than healthy subjects did, regardless of the category. Number of subcategories was also lower in PD patients than in healthy subjects, without a significant difference between categories. Critically, PD patients' sequences were less semantically organized than were those of controls, but this effect appeared in only the smaller category (fruits), thus pointing to a lack of strategy in exploring the semantic store. CONCLUSIONS: Our results show that the semantic fluency deficit in PD patients has a strategic component, even though that may not be the only cause of the impaired performance. Furthermore, our evidence suggests that the semantic category used in the test influences performance, hence providing an explanation for the failure by previous studies, which often used large categories such as animals, to detect strategy deficits in PD. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).


Subject(s)
Parkinson Disease , Humans , Memory , Neuropsychological Tests , Parkinson Disease/complications , Semantics , Verbal Behavior
3.
Neuroimage ; 217: 116854, 2020 08 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32334091

ABSTRACT

With practice, humans improve their performance in a task by either optimizing a known strategy or discovering a novel, potentially more fruitful strategy. We investigated the neural processes underlying these two fundamental abilities by applying fMRI in a task with two possible alternative strategies. For analysis we combined time-resolved network analysis with Coherence Density Peak Clustering (Allegra et al., 2017), univariate GLM, and multivariate pattern classification. Converging evidence showed that the posterior portion of the default network, i.e. the precuneus and the angular gyrus bilaterally, has a central role in the optimization of the current strategy. These regions encoded the relevant spatial information, increased the strength of local connectivity as well as the long-distance connectivity with other relevant regions in the brain (e.g., visual cortex, dorsal attention network). The connectivity increase was proportional to performance optimization. By contrast, the anterior portion of the default network (i.e. medial prefrontal cortex) and the rostral portion of the fronto-parietal network were associated with new strategy discovery: an early increase of local and long-range connectivity centered on these regions was only observed in the subjects who would later shift to a new strategy. Overall, our findings shed light on the dynamic interactions between regions related to attention and with cognitive control, underlying the balance between strategy exploration and exploitation. Results suggest that the default network, far from being "shut-down" during task performance, has a pivotal role in the background exploration and monitoring of potential alternative courses of action.


Subject(s)
Brain/diagnostic imaging , Brain/physiology , Nerve Net/diagnostic imaging , Nerve Net/physiology , Adult , Algorithms , Attention/physiology , Brain Mapping , Cognition/physiology , Decision Making/physiology , Exploratory Behavior/physiology , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Male , Neural Pathways/diagnostic imaging , Neural Pathways/physiology , Neuroimaging/methods , Parietal Lobe/diagnostic imaging , Parietal Lobe/physiology , Prefrontal Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Young Adult
4.
Neuroimage ; 177: 108-116, 2018 08 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29753107

ABSTRACT

For everyday communication, bilingual speakers need to face the complex task of rapidly choosing the most appropriate language given the context, maintaining this choice over the current communicative act, and shielding lexical selection from competing alternatives from non-target languages. Yet, speech production of bilinguals is typically flawless and fluent. Most of the studies available to date constrain speakers' language choice by cueing the target language and conflate language choice with language use. This left largely unexplored the neural mechanisms underlying free language choice, i.e., the voluntary situation of choosing the language to speak. In this study, we used fMRI and Multivariate Pattern Analysis to identify brain regions encoding the target language when bilinguals are free to choose in which language to name pictures. We found that the medial prefrontal cortex encoded the chosen language prior to speaking. By contrast, during language use, language control recruited a wider brain network including the left inferior frontal lobe, the basal ganglia, and the angular and inferior parietal gyrus bilaterally. None of these regions were involved in language choice. We argue that the control processes involved in language choice are different from those involved in language use. Furthermore, our findings confirm that the medial prefrontal cortex is a domain-general region critical for free choice and that bilingual language choice relies on domain general processes.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping/methods , Choice Behavior/physiology , Executive Function/physiology , Multilingualism , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology , Psycholinguistics , Verbal Behavior/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Young Adult
5.
J Cogn Neurosci ; 29(7): 1147-1161, 2017 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28253076

ABSTRACT

An important question for understanding the neural basis of problem solving is whether the regions of human prefrontal cortices play qualitatively different roles in the major cognitive restructuring required to solve difficult problems. However, investigating this question using neuroimaging faces a major dilemma: either the problems do not require major cognitive restructuring, or if they do, the restructuring typically happens once, rendering repeated measurements of the critical mental process impossible. To circumvent these problems, young adult participants were challenged with a one-dimensional Subtraction (or Nim) problem [Bouton, C. L. Nim, a game with a complete mathematical theory. The Annals of Mathematics, 3, 35-39, 1901] that can be tackled using two possible strategies. One, often used initially, is effortful, slow, and error-prone, whereas the abstract solution, once achieved, is easier, quicker, and more accurate. Behaviorally, success was strongly correlated with sex. Using voxel-based morphometry analysis controlling for sex, we found that participants who found the more abstract strategy (i.e., Solvers) had more gray matter volume in the anterior medial, ventrolateral prefrontal, and parietal cortices compared with those who never switched from the initial effortful strategy (i.e., Explorers). Removing the sex covariate showed higher gray matter volume in Solvers (vs. Explorers) in the right ventrolateral prefrontal and left parietal cortex.


Subject(s)
Prefrontal Cortex/physiology , Problem Solving/physiology , Female , Games, Experimental , Gray Matter/diagnostic imaging , Gray Matter/physiology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Mathematical Concepts , Neuropsychological Tests , Organ Size , Prefrontal Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Reaction Time , Sex Characteristics , Young Adult
6.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 38(3): 1421-1437, 2017 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27879036

ABSTRACT

There is growing interest in the description of short-lived patterns in the spatiotemporal cortical activity monitored via neuroimaging. Most traditional analysis methods, designed to estimate relatively long-term brain dynamics, are not always appropriate to capture these patterns. Here we introduce a novel data-driven approach for detecting short-lived fMRI brain activity patterns. Exploiting Density Peak Clustering (Rodriguez and Laio [2014]), our approach reveals well localized clusters by identifying and grouping together voxels whose time-series are similar, irrespective of their brain location, even when very short time windows (∼10 volumes) are used. The method, which we call Coherence Density Peak Clustering (CDPC), is first tested on simulated data and compared with a standard unsupervised approach for fMRI analysis, independent component analysis (ICA). CDPC identifies activated voxels with essentially no false-positives and proves more reliable than ICA, which is troubled by a number of false positives comparable to that of true positives. The reliability of the method is demonstrated on real fMRI data from a simple motor task, containing brief iterations of the same movement. The clusters identified are found in regions expected to be involved in the task, and repeat synchronously with the paradigm. The methodology proposed is especially suitable for the study of short-time brain dynamics and single trial experiments, where the event or task of interest cannot be repeated for the same subject, as happens, for instance, in problem-solving, learning and decision-making. A GUI implementation of our method is available for download at https://github.com/micheleallegra/CDPC. Hum Brain Mapp 38:1421-1437, 2017. © 2016 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping , Brain/diagnostic imaging , Brain/physiology , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Neural Pathways/diagnostic imaging , Neural Pathways/physiology , Adult , Computer Simulation , Female , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Male , Middle Aged , Models, Neurological , Movement/physiology , Oxygen/blood , Principal Component Analysis , Reproducibility of Results , Time Factors , Young Adult
7.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 42(7): 1127-39, 2016 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26820498

ABSTRACT

Our native tongue influences the way we perceive other languages. But does it also determine the way we perceive nonlinguistic sounds? The authors investigated how speakers of Italian, Turkish, and Persian group sequences of syllables, tones, or visual shapes alternating in either frequency or duration. We found strong native listening effects with linguistic stimuli. Speakers of Italian grouped the linguistic stimuli differently from speakers of Turkish and Persian. However, speakers of all languages showed the same perceptual biases when grouping the nonlinguistic auditory and the visual stimuli. The shared perceptual biases appear to be determined by universal grouping principles, and the linguistic differences caused by prosodic differences between the languages. Although previous findings suggest that acquired linguistic knowledge can either enhance or diminish the perception of both linguistic and nonlinguistic auditory stimuli, we found no transfer of native listening effects across auditory domains or perceptual modalities. (PsycINFO Database Record


Subject(s)
Auditory Perception/physiology , Linguistics , Pitch Perception/physiology , Transfer, Psychology/physiology , Voice Quality/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation , Adolescent , Adult , Chi-Square Distribution , Cross-Cultural Comparison , Female , Humans , Iran , Italy , Male , Photic Stimulation , Speech/physiology , Turkey , Young Adult
8.
Front Psychol ; 6: 1183, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26321994

ABSTRACT

Word orders are not distributed equally: SOV and SVO are the most prevalent among the world's languages. While there is a consensus that SOV might be the "default" order in human languages, the factors that trigger the preference for SVO are still a matter of debate. Here we provide a new perspective on word order preferences that emphasizes the role of a lexicon. We propose that while there is a tendency to favor SOV in the case of improvised communication, the exposure to a shared lexicon makes it possible to liberate sufficient cognitive resources to use syntax. Consequently SVO, the more efficient word order to express syntactic relations, emerges. To test this hypothesis, we taught Italian (SVO) and Persian (SOV) speakers a set of gestures and later asked them to describe simple events. Confirming our prediction, results showed that in both groups a consistent use of SVO emerged after acquiring a stable gesture repertoire.

9.
Brain Lang ; 144: 1-9, 2015 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25868150

ABSTRACT

Bilinguals require a high degree of cognitive control to select the language intended for speaking and inhibit the unintended. Previous neuroimaging studies have not teased apart brain regions for generating the intention to use a given language, and those for speaking in that language. Separating these two phases can clarify at what stage competition between languages occurs. In this fMRI study German-English bilinguals were first cued to use German or English. After a delay, they named a picture in the cued language. During the intention phase, the precuneus, right superior lateral parietal lobule, and middle temporal gyrus were more activated when participants had to update the currently active language. During language execution activation was higher for English compared to German in brain areas associated with cognitive control, most notably the anterior cingulate and the caudate. Our results suggest two different systems enabling cognitive control during bilingual language production.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Intention , Language , Multilingualism , Speech/physiology , Brain Mapping , Cognition/physiology , Cues , England , Female , Germany , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Photic Stimulation , Young Adult
10.
Neuropsychologia ; 50(7): 1521-35, 2012 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22433287

ABSTRACT

Planning is essential for normal daily activities. Although the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) is thought to be crucially involved in planning, it remains to be understood whether this contribution is attributable to working memory requirements of the tasks and when it occurs, whether during initial planning or during subsequent plan execution. Here, we compared patterns of activation observed when participants planned and executed their plans to solve Tower of Hanoi problems to when they had to memorize and reproduce externally presented sequences of moves. The DLPFC was preferentially active during initial planning relative to both plan execution and initial memorization of sequences of moves. By contrast, plan execution relied on posterior temporal areas, inferior frontal regions and the dorsolateral premotor cortex. We attribute activation in DLPFC to generation and evaluation of abstract sequences of responses, and activation in the regions underlying plan execution to rehearsal of planned sequences of moves.


Subject(s)
Brain Mapping , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology , Problem Solving/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Female , Humans , Image Processing, Computer-Assisted , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Neural Pathways/blood supply , Neural Pathways/physiology , Neuropsychological Tests , Oxygen/blood , Prefrontal Cortex/blood supply , Reaction Time , Young Adult
11.
J Neurosci ; 31(21): 7763-74, 2011 May 25.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21613489

ABSTRACT

Despite the recent interest in the neuroanatomy of inductive reasoning processes, the regional specificity within prefrontal cortex (PFC) for the different mechanisms involved in induction tasks remains to be determined. In this study, we used fMRI to investigate the contribution of PFC regions to rule acquisition (rule search and rule discovery) and rule following. Twenty-six healthy young adult participants were presented with a series of images of cards, each consisting of a set of circles numbered in sequence with one colored blue. Participants had to predict the position of the blue circle on the next card. The rules that had to be acquired pertained to the relationship among succeeding stimuli. Responses given by subjects were categorized in a series of phases either tapping rule acquisition (responses given up to and including rule discovery) or rule following (correct responses after rule acquisition). Mid-dorsolateral PFC (mid-DLPFC) was active during rule search and remained active until successful rule acquisition. By contrast, rule following was associated with activation in temporal, motor, and medial/anterior prefrontal cortex. Moreover, frontopolar cortex (FPC) was active throughout the rule acquisition and rule following phases before a rule became familiar. We attributed activation in mid-DLPFC to hypothesis generation and in FPC to integration of multiple separate inferences. The present study provides evidence that brain activation during inductive reasoning involves a complex network of frontal processes and that different subregions respond during rule acquisition and rule following phases.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Problem Solving/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Reaction Time/physiology , Adult , Brain Mapping/methods , Female , Humans , Male , Young Adult
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...