Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 19 de 19
Filter
1.
Conscious Cogn ; 82: 102945, 2020 07.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32422548

ABSTRACT

Distortions in sensory experiences that precede a migraine attack have been extensively documented, the most well-known being the visual aura. Distortions in the experience of other senses are also reported as part of an aura, albeit less frequently, together with changes in the perception or ownership of the body or body parts. There are many examples of differences in aspects of visual perception between migraine and control groups, between attacks, but not as much on unusual experiences involving other senses, the sense of the body or the experience of the environment. Seventy-seven migraine (33 with aura) and 74 control participants took part. Anomalous perceptions were experienced by both migraine and control groups, but more with migraine experienced them and rated them as more distressing, intrusive and frequent. Associations with reports of visual triggers of migraine and visual discomfort are presented. This study is the first to show relationships between these factors.


Subject(s)
Migraine Disorders/physiopathology , Perceptual Disorders/physiopathology , Sensation Disorders/physiopathology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Migraine Disorders/complications , Neuropsychological Tests , Perceptual Disorders/diagnosis , Perceptual Disorders/etiology , Sensation Disorders/diagnosis , Sensation Disorders/etiology
2.
Vision (Basel) ; 3(2)2019 Mar 27.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31735813

ABSTRACT

Visual tests can be used as noninvasive tools to test models of the pathophysiology underlying neurological conditions, such as migraine. They may also be used to track changes in performance that vary with the migraine cycle or can track the efficacy of prophylactic treatments. This article reviews the literature on performance differences on two visual tasks, global motion discrimination and orientation, which, of the many visual tasks that have been used to compare differences between migraine and control groups, have yielded the most consistent patterns of group differences. The implications for understanding the underlying pathophysiology in migraine are discussed, but the main focus is on bringing together disparate areas of research and suggesting those that can reveal practical uses of visual tests to treat and manage migraine.

3.
Vision Res ; 156: 73-83, 2019 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30664883

ABSTRACT

This study compared the effects of the colour and size of overlays on reading time, reading errors and on the clarity of text with young primary school children. The sample comprised a non-clinical, typical, sample from an East London primary school. One hundred and six children aged between four and seven years were asked to read 11 short passages of text (60 words) either with full page overlays or smaller reading rulers (53 in each group). This sample included younger children than has often been tested before. The 11 short passages allowed an assessment of baseline reading performance (no reading aid) and performance while reading with each of a set of ten coloured reading aids. Two different, yet beneficial, colours were determined: the most effective and the clearest/most comfortable. Both of these measures are not usually recorded. All but four children had reduced reading times with one of the reading aids and all but one reported their aid improved the perceived visual clarity of the text: the size of the reading aid did not affect reading time or visual clarity significantly. The numbers of skipped words and errors/mis-read words also decreased when reading with the most effective and most comfortable reading aid. Near visual acuity was assessed with and without each child's most effective coloured aid. The most effective aid improved acuity in over a third of the children. Acuity has not been assessed in previous studies. As reported previously, different colours helped different children. In conclusion, coloured reading overlays reduced reading times on the reading test employed here and the size of the reading aid was not crucial to facilitate performance. The largest reductions occurred for the youngest readers, suggesting these aids may be particularly effective for early readers.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Reading , Sensory Aids , Size Perception/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology , Child , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Male , Visual Acuity/physiology
4.
Vision Res ; 151: 31-40, 2018 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29530744

ABSTRACT

This study explored associations between local and global shape perception on coloured backgrounds, colour discrimination, and non-verbal IQ (NVIQ). Five background colours were chosen for the local and global shape tasks that were tailored for the cone-opponent pathways early in the visual system (cardinal colour directions: L-M, loosely, reddish-greenish; and S-(L + M), or tritan colours, loosely, blueish-yellowish; where L, M and S refer to the long, middle and short wavelength sensitive cones). Participants also completed the Farnsworth-Munsell 100-hue test (FM100) to determine whether performance on the local and global shape tasks correlated with colour discrimination overall, or with performance on the L-M and tritan subsets of the FM100 test. Overall performance on the local and global shape tasks did correlate with scores on the FM100 tests, despite the colour of the background being irrelevant to the shape tasks. There were also significantly larger associations between scores for the L-M subset of the FM100 test, compared to the tritan subset, and accuracy on some of the shape tasks on the reddish, greenish and neutral backgrounds. Participants also completed the non-verbal components of the WAIS and the SPM+ version of Raven's progressive matrices, to determine whether performance on the FM100 test, and on the local and global shape tasks, correlated with NVIQ. FM100 scores correlated significantly with both WAIS and SPM+ scores. These results extend previous work that has indicated FM100 performance is not purely a measure of colour discrimination, but also involves aspects of each participant's NVIQ, such as the ability to attend to local and global aspects of the test, part-whole relationships, perceptual organisation and good visuomotor skills. Overall performance on the local and global shape tasks correlated only with the WAIS scores, not the SPM+. These results indicate that those aspects of NVIQ that engage spatial comprehension of local-global relationships and manual manipulation (WAIS), rather than more abstract reasoning (SPM+), are related to performance on the local and global shape tasks. Links are presented between various measures of NVIQ and performance on visual tasks, but they are currently seldom addressed in studies of either shape or colour perception. Further studies to explore these issues are recommended.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Form Perception/physiology , Intelligence/physiology , Adult , Color Perception Tests , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells/physiology , Young Adult
5.
Cephalalgia ; 37(4): 315-326, 2017 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27106927

ABSTRACT

Background Visual after-effects are illusions that occur after prolonged viewing of visual displays. The motion after-effect (MAE), for example, is an illusory impression of motion after viewing moving displays: subsequently, stationary displays appear to drift in the opposite direction. After-effects have been used extensively in basic vision research and in clinical settings, and are enhanced in migraine. Objective The objective of this article is to assess associations between ( 1 ) MAE duration and visual symptoms experienced during/between migraine/headache attacks, and ( 2 ) visual stimuli reported as migraine/headache triggers. Methods The MAE was elicited after viewing motion for 45 seconds. MAE duration was tested for three test contrast displays (high, medium, low). Participants also completed a headache questionnaire that included migraine/headache triggers. Results For each test contrast, the MAE was prolonged in migraine. MAE duration was associated with photophobia; visual triggers (flicker, striped patterns); and migraine or headache frequency. Conclusions Group differences on various visual tasks have been attributed to abnormal cortical processing in migraine, such as hyperexcitability, heightened responsiveness and/or a lack of intra-cortical inhibition. The results are not consistent with hyperexcitability simply from a general lack of inhibition. Alternative multi-stage models are discussed and suggestions for further research are recommended, including visual tests in clinical assessments/clinical trials.


Subject(s)
Illusions/etiology , Illusions/physiology , Migraine Disorders/physiopathology , Motion Perception/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation/methods , Young Adult
6.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 57(3): 1228-34, 2016 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26978029

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: Visual tests can be used as noninvasive tools to test models of the pathophysiology underlying neurological conditions, such as migraine. For example, there are reports that the motion aftereffect, which involves neural processing in several cortical areas, is prolonged in migraine. There also are reports of impaired contrast sensitivity in migraine, however, attributed to a precortical dysfunction. This study explored associations between these two tests of visual function. Specifically, it aimed to clarify whether the magnitude of the motion aftereffect is affected by contrast and contrast sensitivity. METHODS: The motion aftereffect was elicited after observers viewed a coherently moving pattern for 45 seconds. The duration of the subsequent aftereffect was measured with three different test display contrasts (high, medium, low). Contrast sensitivity also was assessed. RESULTS: For each test display contrast, the motion aftereffect was prolonged in migraine compared to the control group. Contrast sensitivity was poorer in the migraine group and was a significant predictor of motion aftereffect duration. CONCLUSIONS: These results suggest an anomaly in early motion processing pathways in migraine that likely is linked with those pathways underlying contrast sensitivity. They provide further evidence for differences in visual processing that begin early, potentially starting at the retina, which have consequences for performance on tasks that putatively examine cortical processing. Differences in precortical and cortical visual pathways are implicated in the pathophysiology underlying migraine.


Subject(s)
Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Migraine Disorders/physiopathology , Motion Perception/physiology , Vision Disorders/physiopathology , Visual Cortex/physiopathology , Visual Pathways/physiopathology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Migraine Disorders/diagnosis , Motion , Vision Disorders/diagnosis , Vision Disorders/etiology , Vision Tests , Young Adult
7.
Biol Sex Differ ; 6: 10, 2015.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25964851

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Sex differences in incidence of cardiovascular disease may reflect age-associated intravascular cellular activation resulting in shedding of cell membrane-derived bioactive microvesicles (MV or microparticles) into the blood. Concentrations of cell-specific MV in blood have the potential to be a diagnostic/prognostic marker of pathology, but ranges of MV must first be established in healthy individuals. This study identified cellular origin of blood-borne MV >0.2 µm in blood of apparently healthy women and men aged from 20-70 years. METHODS: Venous blood from apparently healthy participants in the Mayo Clinic Biobank was collected into tubes containing protease inhibitors as the anticoagulant. MV were isolated by standardized differential centrifugation and characterized by digital flow cytometer. Each cellular origin of MV was verified by two different antibodies with strong correlation between the two distinct antibodies (e.g., for platelet-derived MV, r (2) = 0.97). RESULTS: MV derived from platelets were the most abundant type of MV in blood from women and men in all age groups. Total numbers of phosphatidylserine, P-selectin, and platelet- and endothelium-derived MV were significantly (P < 0.05) greater in women than men. Numbers of MV from erythrocytes and stem/progenitor cells were significantly lower in premenopausal women than age-matched men. Number of tissue factor pathway inhibitor positive MV were significantly (P < 0.05) lower whereas erythrocyte-derived MV were significantly higher in postmenopausal women compared to premenopausal women. In women, there was a positive relationship between age and erythrocyte-derived MV (ρ = 0.28; P = 0.009), while in men adipocyte-derived MV increased with age (ρ = 0.33; P = 0.01). CONCLUSIONS: This study provides ranges for cellular origin of blood-borne MV in age-matched, apparently healthy women and men from which to compare diagnostic and prognostic uses of blood-borne MV in larger studies and patient population. In addition, sex- and age-specific differences in phosphatidylserine, platelet-, endothelium-, erythrocyte-, and adipocyte-derived blood-borne MV may contribute to differential progression of cardiovascular disease in women compared to men.

8.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 55(4): 2539-46, 2014 Apr 17.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24677099

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: People with migraine are relatively poor at judging the direction of motion of coherently moving signal dots when interspersed with noise dots drifting in random directions, a task known as motion coherence. Although this has been taken as evidence of impoverished global pooling of motion signals, it could also arise from unreliable coding of local direction (of each dot), or an inability to segment signal from noise (noise-exclusion). The aim of this study was to determine how these putative limits contribute to impoverished motion processing in migraine. METHODS: Twenty-two participants with migraine (mean age, 34.7 ± 8.3 years; 16 female) and 22 age- and sex-matched controls (mean age, 34.4 ± 6.2 years) performed a motion-coherence task and a motion-equivalent noise task, the latter quantifying local and global limits on motion processing. In addition, participants were tested on analogous equivalent noise paradigms involving judgments of orientation and size, so that the specificity of any findings (to visual dimension) could be ascertained. RESULTS: Participants with migraine exhibited higher motion-coherence thresholds than controls (P = 0.01, independent t-test). However, this difference could not be attributed to deficits in either local or global processing since they performed normally on all equivalent noise tasks (P > 0.05, multivariate ANOVA). CONCLUSIONS: These findings indicate that motion perception in the participants with migraine was limited by an inability to exclude visual noise. We suggest that this is a defining characteristic of visual dysfunction in migraine, a theory that has the potential to integrate a wide range of findings in the literature.


Subject(s)
Motion Perception/physiology , Noise , Sensory Thresholds/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Migraine Disorders/physiopathology , Photic Stimulation , Young Adult
9.
Headache ; 53(7): 1087-103, 2013.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23464876

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To assess the potential for particular colors to alleviate visual discomfort when people with migraine view repetitive geometric or striped patterns. BACKGROUND: Visual stimuli, such as flicker, glare, or stripes, can trigger migraine and headache. They can also elicit feelings of discomfort and aversion. There are reports that color can be used to decrease the experience of discomfort and reduce migraine frequency. DESIGN/METHODS: Five sets of striped patterns (3, 12 cycles per degree [cpd]) were created using cardinal colors tailored to selectively stimulate the early visual pathways: achromatic (black/white), tritan (black/purple, black/yellow), protan/deutan (black/red, black/green). All had the same high luminance contrast (0.9 Michelson contrast). Twenty-eight migraine (14 migraine with aura, 14 migraine without aura) and 14 control participants rated the discomfort and described the distortions seen in these patterns. They were also assessed for visual migraine/headache triggers, contrast sensitivity, color vision, acuity, stereopsis, visual discomfort from reading, and dyslexia. RESULTS: In the migraine groups, a comparable number of illusions were seen with the 3 and 12 cpd achromatic gratings, whereas in the control group the greatest number was seen with the 3 cpd grating. In the migraine groups only, all 4 colors reduced, to some extent, the number of illusions and 2 decreased the discomfort, particularly for the 12 cpd gratings. There were significant group differences for contrast sensitivity, reported visual migraine/headache triggers, and the visual discomfort scale. There were a few significant correlations between the different measures, notably between the achromatic visual discomfort measures and reports of visual migraine triggers. CONCLUSIONS: Color, independent of luminance or particular color contrasts, can have therapeutic effects for people with visually triggered migraine as it can reduce the number of perceived illusions when viewing stripes or text. The effect was not color-specific and was greatest for the 12 cpd gratings. Given the significant associations between the achromatic discomfort measures and reports of visual triggers, and the lack of significant associations between the chromatic discomfort measures and reports of visual triggers, further research is recommended to explore the potential to reduce the number of visually triggered migraines with color in addition to alleviating visual discomfort.


Subject(s)
Color Perception/physiology , Migraine Disorders/physiopathology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Space Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Migraine Disorders/diagnosis , Migraine Disorders/etiology , Photic Stimulation/adverse effects , Time Factors , Young Adult
10.
Cephalalgia ; 32(7): 554-70, 2012 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22529196

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: There are conflicting reports concerning the ability of people with migraine to detect and discriminate visual motion. Previous studies used different displays and none adequately assessed other parameters that could affect performance, such as those that could indicate precortical dysfunction. METHODS: Motion-direction detection, discrimination and relative motion thresholds were compared from participants with and without migraine. Potentially relevant visual covariates were included (contrast sensitivity; acuity; stereopsis; visual discomfort, stress, triggers; dyslexia). RESULTS: For each task, migraine participants were less accurate than a control group and had impaired contrast sensitivity, greater visual discomfort, visual stress and visual triggers. Only contrast sensitivity correlated with performance on each motion task; it also mediated performance. CONCLUSIONS: Impaired performance on certain motion tasks can be attributed to impaired contrast sensitivity early in the visual system rather than a deficit in cortical motion processing per se. There were, however, additional differences for global and relative motion thresholds embedded in noise, suggesting changes in extrastriate cortex in migraine. Tasks to study the effects of noise on performance at different levels of the visual system and across modalities are recommended. A battery of standard visual tests should be included in any future work on the visual system and migraine.


Subject(s)
Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Dyslexia/physiopathology , Migraine Disorders/physiopathology , Motion Perception/physiology , Vision Disorders/physiopathology , Adolescent , Adult , Artifacts , Depth Perception/physiology , Discrimination, Psychological/physiology , Dyslexia/etiology , Female , Humans , Male , Migraine Disorders/complications , Vision Disorders/etiology , Young Adult
11.
Cephalalgia ; 31(3): 346-56, 2011 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20813782

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: In visual metacontrast masking, the visibility of a brief target stimulus can be reduced substantially if it is preceded (forward masking) or followed (backward masking) by a non-overlapping mask. These effects have been attributed to inhibitory processes within the visual system. Two previous studies have used metacontrast masking to assess inhibitory function in migraine and control groups, however, each used different types of masking and obtained different results. SUBJECTS AND METHODS: Forward, backward and combined forward and backward masking were compared in migraine (15 with visual aura, 15 without) and control (n = 15) groups. Baseline trials were also included (target only). RESULTS: For all types of masking, both migraine groups were more accurate than the control group. When performance for the masking trials was expressed relative to baseline, however, there were no significant group differences. Performance in certain conditions nevertheless correlated significantly with migraine frequency and with the recency of the last attack. CONCLUSIONS: The inhibitory processes involved in the masking tasks employed in this study do not appear to be impaired in migraine. Their better overall performance may reflect a sensitivity difference, perhaps as a consequence of a heightened neuronal response, which varies with the migraine cycle.


Subject(s)
Migraine Disorders/physiopathology , Perceptual Masking/physiology , Adult , Humans , Middle Aged , Photic Stimulation , Young Adult
12.
Pain ; 147(1-3): 132-40, 2009 Dec 15.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19796876

ABSTRACT

In adult patients with migraine, transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) has been used to examine cortical excitability between attacks, but there have been discrepant results. No TMS study has examined cortical excitability in children or adolescents with migraine. Here, we employed TMS to study regional excitability of the occipital (phosphene threshold [PT] and suppression of visual perception) and motor (resting motor threshold and cortical silent period) cortex in ten children suffering from migraine without aura and ten healthy age-matched controls. Patients were studied 1-2 days before and after a migraine attack as well as during the inter-migraine interval. The motion aftereffect was also investigated at each time-point as an index of cortical reactivity to moving visual stimuli. Migraineurs had lower PTs compared to healthy participants at each time-point, indicating increased occipital excitability. This increase in occipital excitability was attenuated 1-2 days before a migraine attack as indicated by a relative increase in PTs. The increase in PTs before the next attack was associated with a stronger TMS-induced suppression of visual perception and a prolongation of the motion aftereffect. Motor cortex excitability was not altered in patients and did not change during the migraine cycle. These findings show that pediatric migraine without aura is associated with a systematic shift in occipital excitability preceding the migraine attack. Similar systematic fluctuations in cortical excitability might be present in adult migraineurs and may reflect either a protective mechanism or an abnormal decrease in cortical excitability that predisposes an individual to a migraine attack.


Subject(s)
Cerebral Cortex/physiopathology , Evoked Potentials, Motor/physiology , Migraine without Aura/pathology , Migraine without Aura/physiopathology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Analysis of Variance , Case-Control Studies , Child , Databases, Factual/statistics & numerical data , Electric Stimulation/methods , Female , Humans , Male , Motion Perception/physiology , Statistics as Topic , Time Factors , Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation/methods
13.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 50(3): 1470-6, 2009 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19029037

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: Photosensitivity, or photoparoxysmal response (PPR), is an abnormal EEG reaction to intermittent photic stimulation (IPS), consisting of spikes, spike-waves, and intermittent slow waves. Depending on the spread of the abnormal activity, PPR subgroups have been defined as having either propagating PPR or localized, occipital spikes (OS) only. Recent research suggests an enhanced excitability in the occipital cortex may underlie this reaction during IPS, but it remains unclear whether changes in excitability affect the function of the occipital cortex to other, less provocative visual stimuli. In this study, cortical function in photosensitivity was assessed using two visual aftereffects that occur after prolonged adaptation. METHODS: Motion and tilt aftereffects were compared in healthy subjects with (n = 14, seven with propagating PPR, seven with OS) or without (n = 14) PPR. RESULTS: The duration of the motion aftereffect was shorter in the PPR group than in the control group. The size of the tilt aftereffect did not differ between the groups. Thirteen from each group had participated in an earlier study in which occipital transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) was used to elicit phosphenes and to suppress the perception of briefly presented letters. The TMS intensity required to elicit phosphenes correlated with the size of the tilt aftereffect (TAE) in the PPR group only. CONCLUSIONS: This study provides further evidence of enhanced cortical excitability in subjects with photosensitivity, which is likely to reflect changes in excitatory neurotransmission.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Ocular/physiology , Ocular Motility Disorders/physiopathology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Photic Stimulation/adverse effects , Visual Cortex/physiopathology , Visual Perception/physiology , Adolescent , Adult , Afterimage/physiology , Child , Electroencephalography , Evoked Potentials, Motor/physiology , Evoked Potentials, Visual/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Motion Perception , Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation
14.
Biol Lett ; 4(5): 446-8, 2008 Oct 23.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18682357

ABSTRACT

This study is the first to demonstrate that human yawns are possibly contagious to domestic dogs (Canis familiaris). Twenty-nine dogs observed a human yawning or making control mouth movements. Twenty-one dogs yawned when they observed a human yawning, but control mouth movements did not elicit yawning from any of them. The presence of contagious yawning in dogs suggests that this phenomenon is not specific to primate species and may indicate that dogs possess the capacity for a rudimentary form of empathy. Since yawning is known to modulate the levels of arousal, yawn contagion may help coordinate dog-human interaction and communication. Understanding the mechanism as well as the function of contagious yawning between humans and dogs requires more detailed investigation.


Subject(s)
Imitative Behavior , Yawning , Animals , Dogs , Empathy , Female , Humans , Male
15.
Brain ; 130(Pt 1): 78-87, 2007 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17121743

ABSTRACT

Photosensitivity or photoparoxysmal response (PPR) is a highly heritable electroencephalographic trait characterized by an abnormal cortical response to intermittent photic stimulation (IPS). In PPR-positive individuals, IPS induces spikes, spike-waves or intermittent slow waves. The PPR may be restricted to posterior visual areas (i.e. local PPR with occipital spikes only) or spread to anterior non-visual cortical regions (i.e. PPR with propagation). The mechanisms underlying the PPR and causing its spread remain to be clarified. In unmedicated PPR-positive individuals and PPR-negative control participants without any history of previous seizures, we used focal transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to investigate the excitability of the visual or primary motor cortex (M1). In the first experiment [18 healthy control subjects (i.e. without PPR in electroencephalography: 6 females, mean age 26.5 +/- 7.34 years) and 17 healthy participants with PPR (7 females, mean age 25.18 +/- 12.2 years) were studied], occipital TMS was used to elicit phosphenes or to suppress the visual perception of letter trigrams. PPR-positive individuals with propagation had lower phosphene thresholds and steeper stimulus-response curves than individuals without PPR or with occipital spikes only. Occipital TMS also induced a stronger suppression of visual perception in PPR-positive subjects with propagation relative to subjects without PPR or with occipital spikes. In the second experiment, we applied TMS over the right M1 without concurrent IPS and measured the motor threshold, the stimulus response curve, and the duration of the cortical silent period (CSP) in PPR positive individuals with propagation and in PPR-negative control participants [15 right-handed healthy subjects without PPR (3 males, mean age 17.7 +/- 3.6 years) and 14 right-handed healthy individuals showing a PPR with propagation (3 males, mean age 17.4 +/- 3.9 years)]. PPR-positive individuals showed no changes in these excitability measures relative to the PPR-negative control participants. We also measured the modifiability of the CSP by continuous IPS at a frequency of 18 or 50 Hz. While IPS reduced the duration of the CSP in PPR-negative control subjects, IPS had no effect on the duration of the CSP in PPR-positive individuals. Our results provide first time evidence that the propagation of the PPR is associated with increased excitability of the occipital but not the motor cortex. The stronger inhibitory effect of TMS on visual perception and the failure of IPS to shorten the CSP in PPR-positive participants may possibly reflect adaptive changes that prevent the provocation of seizures during the PPR.


Subject(s)
Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Electroencephalography/methods , Photic Stimulation/adverse effects , Adolescent , Adult , Evoked Potentials, Motor/physiology , Evoked Potentials, Visual/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Motor Cortex/physiology , Phosphenes/physiology , Sensory Thresholds , Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation/methods , Visual Cortex/physiology , Visual Perception/physiology
16.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 47(12): 5599-604, 2006 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17122154

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: To determine whether orientation discrimination deficits in migraine, which have been found to depend on the spatial frequency of the stimulus, are due to precortical dysfunction or to abnormal patterns of orientation tuning at cortical loci. Further, to assess whether any cortical involvement is restricted to the striate cortex or whether higher cortical areas are also involved. Orientation-specific abnormalities would provide evidence of cortical dysfunction. METHODS: Orientation-discrimination and contrast-detection thresholds were assessed at cardinal (0 degrees) and oblique (45 degrees) orientations using explicit lines defined by Gabor patches. To test for extrastriate dysfunction, participants made orientation judgments using virtual lines defined by two widely spaced circles. Migraine history, migraine triggers, and pattern sensitivity were also assessed. Twenty migraineurs (10 with visual aura, 10 without) and 20 control participants were tested. RESULTS: Orientation-discrimination thresholds were lower for discriminations made about the cardinal axis than for discriminations made about the oblique axis, a well-documented phenomenon known as the oblique effect. Relative to the control group, the migraine group exhibited orientation-specific sensitivity losses on explicit and virtual judgments. Orientation-discrimination thresholds about the oblique axis were significantly elevated in the migraine group. In contrast, the migraine and control groups' detection thresholds did not differ. CONCLUSIONS: These findings reflect abnormal function of striate and extrastriate cortex in migraine. In addition, the discrimination data are consistent with wider orientation-tuning curves for orientation-sensitive cells in migraine, whereas the detection data suggest peak sensitivity does not differ between the groups.


Subject(s)
Contrast Sensitivity/physiology , Migraine with Aura/physiopathology , Migraine without Aura/physiopathology , Orientation , Vision Disorders/physiopathology , Visual Cortex/physiopathology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Sensory Thresholds/physiology
17.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 47(11): 5125-31, 2006 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17065534

ABSTRACT

PURPOSE: To determine whether the magnitude of transient tritanopia (TT) differs between migraine and control groups. TT is a retinal phenomenon characterized by a paradoxical reduction in sensitivity to short-wavelength (purple) stimuli after extinction of long-wavelength (yellow) adapting displays. A group difference in the magnitude of TT would provide evidence for a retinal contribution to the S-cone-specific color-processing abnormalities that have been reported in migraine. METHODS: Thirty-two migraineurs and 32 age- and sex-matched control participants were tested with a four-alternative, forced-choice procedure to determine S-cone increment and decrement detection thresholds before and after adaptation to a long-wavelength (yellow) display and a neutral (white) display. Migraine history, migraine triggers, and pattern sensitivity were also assessed. RESULTS: Both groups' detection thresholds for increment (purple) S-cone stimuli were increased after extinction of the long-wavelength adapting display compared with the neutral display, demonstrating TT. This loss of sensitivity was significantly greater in the migraine group. In contrast, loss of sensitivity to decrement (yellow) S-cone stimuli was less marked and did not differ between the groups. The magnitude of TT correlated positively with indices of pattern sensitivity and susceptibility to visually triggered migraines but not with migraine history. CONCLUSIONS: These results demonstrate that abnormalities in a specific retinal circuit contribute to decreased short-wavelength sensitivity after adaptation in migraine. As thresholds did not correlate with indices of migraine history, it is unlikely that this finding reflects cumulative damage induced by repeated migraine episodes.


Subject(s)
Color Vision Defects/physiopathology , Migraine Disorders/physiopathology , Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells/physiopathology , Visual Pathways/physiopathology , Adult , Color Perception , Color Perception Tests , Female , Humans , Male , Sensory Thresholds , Surveys and Questionnaires , Visual Fields
18.
Headache ; 46(9): 1431-40, 2006 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17040340

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To investigate the associations between interictal pattern glare, visual stress, and visual triggers of migraine. BACKGROUND: There has been relatively little research on the visual stimuli that can trigger migraine episodes. This is surprising, since if practitioners can obviate such triggers, then some attacks may be prevented. The existing literature suggests that patients who are prone to visually triggered migraines report more illusions on viewing striped patterns ("pattern glare") and that colored filters may be an effective intervention for these people. METHODS: Headache symptoms and headache triggers were investigated in migraine and control groups in 2 separate experiments. In one experiment, we also determined, for each participant, pattern glare, whether it was reduced by colored filters and, if so, what the optimum color of filter was. Color vision was also assessed with the D15 test. RESULTS: People with migraine saw significantly more illusions on viewing each striped pattern and experienced greater pattern glare. They were also more likely to select a colored filter to aid visual comfort, particularly colors in the blue-to-green sector of the spectrum. Color vision was impaired subtly but significantly in migraine. Principal component analyses grouped common headache triggers into 5 broadly equal components: food, visual triggers, alcohol, stress and tiredness, and the environment. In a second analysis, the overall number of illusions seen in striped patterns was associated with visual triggers while pattern glare, use of colored filters, and interictal light sensitivity together formed a sixth component interpreted as visual stress. CONCLUSIONS: It is suggested that clinicians should ask migraine patients whether visual stimuli trigger their migraine, about interictal visual symptoms, and use the pattern glare test to ensure that those who may benefit from optometric interventions are appropriately managed.


Subject(s)
Fructose/analogs & derivatives , Migraine Disorders/drug therapy , Migraine Disorders/prevention & control , Neuroprotective Agents/therapeutic use , Serotonin Antagonists/therapeutic use , Tryptamines/therapeutic use , Acute Disease , Adult , Female , Fructose/therapeutic use , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Migraine Disorders/epidemiology , Pilot Projects , Secondary Prevention , Topiramate
19.
Headache ; 46(4): 611-21, 2006 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16643556

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To examine visual search performance in migraine and headache-free control groups and to determine whether reports of selective color vision deficits in migraine occur preattentively. BACKGROUND: Visual search is a classic technique to measure certain components of visual attention. The technique can be manipulated to measure both preattentive (automatic) and attentive processes. Here, visual search for colored targets was employed to extend earlier reports that the detection or discrimination of colors selective for the short-wavelength sensitive cone photoreceptors in the retina (S or "blue" cones) is impaired in migraine. METHOD: Visual search performance for small and large color differences was measured in 34 migraine and 34 control participants. Small and large color differences were included to assess attentive and preattentive processing, respectively. In separate conditions, colored stimuli were chosen that would be detected selectively by either the S-, or by the long- (L or "red") and middle (M or "green")-wavelength sensitive cone photoreceptors. RESULTS: The results showed no preattentive differences between the migraine and control groups. For active, or attentive, search, differences between the migraine and control groups occurred for colors detected by the S-cones only, there were no differences for colors detected by the L- and M-cones. The migraine group responded significantly more slowly than the control group for the S-cone colors. CONCLUSIONS: The pattern of results indicates that there are no overall differences in search performance between migraine and control groups. The differences found for the S-cone colors are attributed to impaired discrimination of these colors in migraine and not to differences in attention.


Subject(s)
Migraine Disorders/physiopathology , Vision Disorders/physiopathology , Vision, Ocular/physiology , Attention , Color Perception Tests , Color Vision Defects/etiology , Color Vision Defects/physiopathology , Migraine Disorders/complications , Retinal Cone Photoreceptor Cells/physiopathology , Vision Disorders/etiology , Visual Fields/physiology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...