Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 24
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Trends Neurosci ; 24(10): 609-16, 2001 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11576676

ABSTRACT

In most birds and mammals, young are raised in family groups. The phenotypes of nestmates and parents are thus reliable cues for recognition of conspecifics and kin. However, in some species, young develop alone, or in broods of mixed relatedness (e.g. because of multiple paternity or maternity), or among heterospecifics or unrelated conspecifics (brood parasites). Under these circumstances, the best referent (model) for discriminating close from distant kin and heterospecifics from conspecifics might be one's own self. This recognition process is known as self-referent phenotype matching. Here we review recent experimental evidence of self-referencing and suggest that behavioral neuroscience can provide new tools and insights into how it works (its proximate mechanistic and ontogenetic bases) and why it exists (its adaptive significance).


Subject(s)
Brain/physiology , Nesting Behavior/physiology , Sexual Behavior, Animal/physiology , Animals , Phenotype , Species Specificity
2.
Evol Hum Behav ; 22(3): 147-163, 2001 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11384883

ABSTRACT

Spices are aromatic plant materials that are used in cooking. Recently it was hypothesized that spice use yields a health benefit: cleansing food of parasites and pathogens before it is eaten, thereby reducing food poisoning and foodborne illnesses. In support, most spices have antimicrobial properties and use of spices in meat-based recipes is greatest in hot climates, where the diversity and growth rates of microorganisms are highest. A critical prediction of the antimicrobial hypothesis is that spices should be used less in preparing vegetables than meat dishes. This is because cells of dead plants are better protected physically and chemically against bacteria and fungi than cells of dead animals (whose immune system ceased functioning at death), so fewer spices would be necessary to make vegetables safe for consumption. We tested this corollary by compiling information on 2129 vegetable-only recipes from 107 traditional cookbooks of 36 countries. Analyses revealed that spice use increased with increasing ambient temperature, but less dramatically than in meat-based recipes. In all 36 countries, vegetable dishes called for fewer spices per recipe than meat dishes; 27 of these differences were significant. Of 41 individual spices, 38 were used less frequently in vegetable recipes; 30 of these differences were significant. Proportions of recipes that called for >1 spice and >1 extremely potent antimicrobial spice also were significantly lower for vegetable dishes. By every measure, vegetable-based recipes were significantly less spicy than meat-based recipes. Within-country analyses control for possible differences in spice plant availability and degrees of cultural independence. Results thus strongly support the antimicrobial hypothesis.

3.
Proc Biol Sci ; 268(1471): 1041-8, 2001 May 22.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11375088

ABSTRACT

Recognition of conspecifics is an essential precursor of sexual reproduction. Most mammals and birds learn salient features of their parents or siblings early in ontogeny and later recognize individuals whose phenotypes match the mental image (template) of relatives closely enough as conspecifics. However, the young of brood parasites are reared among heterospecifics, so social learning will yield inappropriate species recognition templates. Initially, it was inferred that conspecific recognition in brood parasites depended on genetically determined templates. More recently it was demonstrated that learning plays a critical role in the development of parasites' social preferences. Here we propose a mechanism that accommodates the interaction of learned and genetic components of recognition. We suggest that conspecific recognition is initiated when a young parasite encounters some unique species-specific signal or "password" (e.g. a vocalization, behaviour or other characteristic) that triggers learning of additional aspects of the password-giver's phenotype. We examined the possibility that nestlings of the obligately brood-parasitic brown-headed cowbird (Molothrus ater) could use a species-specific vocalization, the "chatter", as a password. We found that six-day-old nestlings responded (begged) significantly more frequently to playbacks of chatters than to other avian sounds and that two-month-old fledglings approached playbacks of chatters more quickly than vocalizations of heterospecifics. Free-living cowbird fledglings and adults also approached playbacks of chatters more often than control sounds. Passwords may be involved in the ontogeny of species recognition in brood parasites generally.


Subject(s)
Songbirds , Vocalization, Animal , Animals , Species Specificity
4.
Q Rev Biol ; 75(2): 113-48, 2000 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10858967

ABSTRACT

Approximately two-thirds of women experience nausea or vomiting during the first trimester of pregnancy. These symptoms are commonly known as morning sickness. Hook (1976) and Profet (1988) hypothesized that morning sickness protects the embryo by causing pregnant women to physically expel and subsequently avoid foods that contain teratogenic and abortifacient chemicals, especially toxic chemicals in strong-tasting vegetables, caffeinated beverages and alcohol. We examined this hypothesis by comprehensively reviewing the relevant medical, psychological and anthropological literature. In its support, (i) symptoms peak when embryonic organogenesis is most susceptible to chemical disruption (weeks 6-18), (ii) women who experience morning sickness are significantly less likely to miscarry than women who do not (9 of 9 studies), (iii) women who vomit suffer fewer miscarriages than those who experience nausea alone, and (iv) many pregnant women have aversions to alcoholic and nonalcoholic (mostly caffeinated) beverages and strong-tasting vegetables, especially during the first trimester. Surprisingly, however, the greatest aversions are to meats, fish, poultry, and eggs. A cross-cultural analysis using the Human Relations Area Files revealed 20 traditional societies in which morning sickness has been observed and seven in which it has never been observed. The latter were significantly less likely to have animal products as dietary staples and significantly more likely to have only plants (primarily corn) as staples than the 20 societies in which morning sickness occurred. Animal products may be dangerous to pregnant women and their embryos because they often contain parasites and pathogens, especially when stored at room temperatures in warm climates. Avoiding foodborne microorganisms is particularly important to pregnant women because they are immunosuppressed, presumably to reduce the chances of rejecting tissues of their own offspring (Haig 1993). As a result, pregnant women are more vulnerable to serious, often deadly infections. We hypothesize that morning sickness causes women to avoid foods that might be dangerous to themselves or their embryos, especially foods that, prior to widespread refrigeration, were likely to be heavily laden with microorganisms and their toxins. The alternative hypotheses that morning sickness is (i) an epiphenomenon of mother-offspring genetic conflict or hormones associated with viable pregnancies, or (ii) an indicator to potential sexual partners and kin that the woman is pregnant, resulting in reduced sexual behavior and increased nepotistic aid, were not well supported. Available data are most consistent with the hypothesis that morning sickness serves an adaptive, prophylactic function.


Subject(s)
Food Contamination , Nausea , Pregnancy Complications/physiopathology , Vomiting , Adaptation, Physiological , Cultural Characteristics , Diet , Embryonic and Fetal Development , Female , Humans , Pregnancy , Temperature
5.
6.
Q Rev Biol ; 73(1): 3-49, 1998 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9586227

ABSTRACT

Although spices have been important for centuries in food preparation throughout the world, patterns of spice use differ considerably among cultures and countries. What factors underlie these differences? Why are spices used at all? To investigate these questions, we quantified the frequency of use of 43 spices in the meat-based cuisines of the 36 countries for which we could locate traditional cookbooks. A total of 4578 recipes from 93 cookbooks was analysed. We also complied information on the temperature and precipitation in each country, the ranges of spice plants, and the antibacterial properties of each spice. These data were used to investigate the hypothesis that spices inhibit or kill food-spoilage microorganisms. In support of this is the fact that spice plant secondary compounds are powerful antimicrobial (i.e., antibacterial and antifungal) agents. As mean annual temperatures (an indicator of relative spoilage rates of unrefrigerated foods) increased, the proportion of recipes containing spices, number of spices per recipe, total number of spices used, and use of the most potent antibacterial spices all increased, both within and among countries. Likewise, the estimated fraction of bacterial species inhibited per recipe in each country was positively correlated with annual temperature. Several alternative hypotheses were considered--that spices provide macronutrients, disguise the taste and smell of spoiled foods, or increase perspiration and thus evaporative cooling; it also is conceivable that spice use provides no benefits. However, none of these four alternatives was well supported by our data. The proximate reason spices are used obviously is to enhance food palatability. But the ultimate reason is most likely that spices help cleanse foods of pathogens and thereby contribute to the health, longevity and reproductive success of people who find their flavors enjoyable.


Subject(s)
Cooking/methods , Food Contamination/prevention & control , Spices/microbiology , Bacteria/drug effects , Climate , Cultural Characteristics , Fungi/drug effects , Humans
7.
Am Nat ; 151(4): 392-6, 1998 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18811329
9.
Trends Ecol Evol ; 11(11): 472, 1996 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21237927
10.
Sci Am ; 272(6): 98-103, 1995 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7761819

ABSTRACT

Many organisms, from sea squirts to primates, can identify their relatives. Understanding how and why they do so has prompted new thinking about the evolution of social behavior.


Subject(s)
Social Behavior , Animals , Biological Evolution , Chemoreceptor Cells/physiology , Cues , Mental Recall/physiology , Smell/physiology
11.
Trends Ecol Evol ; 10(6): 246-7, 1995 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21237025
12.
Trends Ecol Evol ; 10(12): 489, 1995 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21237115
14.
J Hand Ther ; 7(2): 111-21, 1994.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8038874

ABSTRACT

The understanding of early controlled range of motion adapted to the stability of the fracture is crucial in postfracture shoulder therapy. The diagnosis and management of proximal humeral fractures have been reviewed. In addition, the aspects of therapy and the pertinent rehabilitation protocols are outlined. Although this article outlines standard treatment approaches, the specific therapy should be individualized to the patient's goals and abilities, and continuous communication between all persons involved in the patient's care is essential for achieving the best possible outcome.


Subject(s)
Shoulder Fractures/rehabilitation , Exercise Therapy/methods , Female , Fracture Fixation/methods , Humans , Incidence , Male , Postoperative Complications , Range of Motion, Articular , Shoulder Fractures/classification , Shoulder Fractures/diagnosis , Shoulder Fractures/epidemiology , Shoulder Fractures/etiology , Shoulder Fractures/surgery
15.
Trends Ecol Evol ; 9(2): 47-51, 1994 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21236765

ABSTRACT

Comparative studies of two species of mole-rat are helping to clarify the ecological correlates of mammalian eusociality. Both species live in social groups composed of close kin, within which breeding is restricted to one female and one to three males. They inhabit xeric areas with dispersed, patchy food and unpredictable rainfall. During droughts, they can neither expand their tunnel systems nor disperse. In brief periods after rain the animals must cooperate and dig furiously to locate rich food patches. By living in groups, arid-zone mole-rats can take full advantage of windows of opportunity when conditions are right for burrowing. Thus, ecological factors and kin selection have apparently interacted in the evolution of eusociality in these species.

16.
Science ; 255(5041): 217-8, 1992 Jan 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17756071
17.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 87(7): 2496-500, 1990 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2320570

ABSTRACT

Using the technique of DNA fingerprinting, we investigated the genetic structure within and among four wild-caught colonies (n = 50 individuals) of a eusocial mammal, the naked mole-rat (Heterocephalus glaber; Rodentia: Bathyergidae). We found that DNA fingerprints of colony-mates were strikingly similar and that between colonies they were much more alike than fingerprints of non-kin in other free-living vertebrates. Extreme genetic similarity within colonies is due to close genetic relationship (mean relatedness estimate +/- SE, r = 0.81 +/- 0.10), which apparently results from consanguineous mating. The inbreeding coefficient (F = 0.45 +/- 0.18) is the highest yet recorded among wild mammals. The genetic structure of naked mole-rat colonies lends support to kin selection and ecological constraints models for the evolution of cooperative breeding and eusociality.


Subject(s)
DNA/genetics , Inbreeding , Rodentia/genetics , Animals , Brain Chemistry , Crosses, Genetic , DNA/isolation & purification , DNA Probes , Female , Heterozygote , Homozygote , Humans , Liver/analysis , Male , Muscles/analysis , Nucleic Acid Hybridization , Nucleotide Mapping
18.
Nature ; 338(6214): 418-20, 1989 Mar 30.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2927502

ABSTRACT

Following a copulation, males in many species of vertebrates (particularly birds) and invertebrates remain near the inseminated female and repel other suitors with displays or force. Guarding males must delay resumption of competitive mate searching, but they may insure their paternity by reducing possibilities for secondary matings and sperm competition. Among mammals, post-copulatory mate guarding has been reported in rodents, mongooses, ungulates and primates, including humans, but the effects of such behaviour on male reproductive success have not been determined genetically. I report here that mate guarding by male Idaho ground squirrels (Spermophilus brunneus) enhances a male's probability of paternity. Furthermore, an analysis based on game theory shows that mate guarding is an evolutionarily stable strategy for male S. brunneus, but not male Belding's ground squirrels (S. beldingi), which resume searching once copulation is completed.


Subject(s)
Sciuridae/physiology , Sexual Behavior, Animal , Animals , Female , Male , Phenotype , Reproduction
19.
Trends Ecol Evol ; 3(1): 8-13, 1988 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21227051

ABSTRACT

Behavioural ecologists have long assumed that animals discriminate between their kin and non-kin, but paid little attention to how animals recognize their relatives. Although the first papers on kin recognition mechanisms appeared barely 10 years ago, studies now appear frequently in journals of animal behaviour. Initial findings reveal that kin recognition abilities are surprisingly well-distributed throughout the animal kingdom. Yet an understanding of the evolutionary and ecological significance of these abilities demands further analyses of the components of kin recognition mechanisms and the social contexts in which they are expressed. Many controversies and unresolved issues remain, and experimental approaches to these problems promise to continue making kin recognition an important, rapidly moving discipline within behavioural ecology.

20.
Science ; 226(4671): 161-2, 1984 Oct 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17814342
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...