Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 5 de 5
Filter
Add more filters











Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Brain Inj ; 23(6): 528-34, 2009 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19484626

ABSTRACT

PRIMARY OBJECTIVE: This study examines the relationship between scores on the Western Neuro Sensory Stimulation Profile (WNSSP) and therapeutic participation as it relates to rehabilitation readiness (RR) in adolescents with low response following severe traumatic brain injury (TBI). RESEARCH DESIGN: This is a serial observational design using multiple measures of clinical status and participation. METHODS AND PROCEDURES: Ten children, mean age 16.7 years, who remained in a low response state (30 days or more) were assessed with the WNSSP and videotaped during physical and occupational therapy sessions. Associations were evaluated between WNSSP scores and participation scores related to arousal, awareness and communication. MAIN OUTCOMES AND RESULTS: The WNSSP was only associated with the communication score (p < 0.0001). The arousal and awareness scores had no significant impact on the WNSSP score. CONCLUSIONS: These results suggest that scores on the WNSSP may be related to the return of communication skills in adolescents in low response states as one part of assessing their therapeutic participation and ultimate rehabilitation readiness. This ability may assist in making decisions regarding care planning.


Subject(s)
Arousal/physiology , Awareness/physiology , Brain Injuries/rehabilitation , Adolescent , Arousal/drug effects , Awareness/drug effects , Brain Injuries/drug therapy , Child , Female , Humans , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Quality of Life , Recovery of Function , Severity of Illness Index , Treatment Outcome , Young Adult
2.
Int J Cogn Ther ; 2(1): 16-36, 2009.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23243482

ABSTRACT

The literatures examining disgust's role in information processing biases and evidence for memory biases in anxiety disorders are both mixed, suggesting small or fragile effects. Thus, to maximize power and reliability, a mega-analytic approach was used to examine data across two studies to determine whether a memory bias for spider-relevant information exists under conditions when disgust is elevated and if the bias is specific to highly fearful individuals. Disgust was manipulated by having a large tarantula present or absent (at encoding and/or at recall) when individuals high (N=158) or low (N=108) in spider fear completed a free recall task. Results indicated that, as expected, the spider's presence was related to enhanced recall of spider information. However, this bias was driven by the presence of the spider during encoding only (as opposed to during recall), and high trait spider fear was not necessary for the memory bias to be expressed. Finally, there was a small effect for individual differences in trait and state disgust to predict spider recall, but this effect disappeared when anxiety was also included as a predictor, suggesting only a limited role for disgust in memory biases related to specific fears.

3.
J Consult Clin Psychol ; 76(6): 988-1002, 2008 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19045967

ABSTRACT

Cognitive models of anxiety and panic suggest that symptom reduction during treatment should be preceded by changes in cognitive processing, including modifying the anxious schema. The current study tested these hypotheses by using a repeated measures design to evaluate whether the trajectory of change in automatic panic associations over a 12-week course of cognitive behavior therapy (CBT) is related to the trajectory of change in panic symptoms. Individuals with panic disorder (N = 43) completed a measure of automatic panic associations--the Implicit Association Test (A. G. Greenwald, D. E. McGhee, & J. L. K. Schwartz, 1998), which reflects elements of the schema construct--every 3 weeks over the course of therapy and measures of panic symptoms each week. Dynamic bivariate latent difference score modeling not only indicated that automatic panic associations changed over the course of CBT for panic disorder but showed these changes were correlated with symptom reduction. Moreover, change in automatic panic associations was a significant predictor of change in panic symptom severity. These findings permit inferences about the temporality of change, suggesting that cognitive change does in fact precede and contribute to symptom change.


Subject(s)
Association , Automatism , Cognitive Behavioral Therapy/methods , Panic Disorder/psychology , Panic Disorder/therapy , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Panic Disorder/diagnosis , Severity of Illness Index , Surveys and Questionnaires , Young Adult
4.
Behav Res Ther ; 46(11): 1244-50, 2008 Nov.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18804199

ABSTRACT

The current study investigates sudden gains (rapid symptom reduction) in group cognitive-behavioral therapy for panic disorder. Sudden gains occurring after session 2 of treatment predicted overall symptom reduction at treatment termination and some changes in cognitive biases. Meanwhile, sudden gains occurring immediately following session 1 were not associated with symptom reduction or cognitive change. Together, this research points to the importance of examining sudden gains across the entire span of treatment, as well as the potential role of sudden gains in recovery from panic disorder.


Subject(s)
Cognitive Behavioral Therapy/methods , Panic Disorder/therapy , Adult , Female , Follow-Up Studies , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Panic Disorder/psychology , Psychiatric Status Rating Scales , Psychotherapy, Group/methods , Treatment Outcome
5.
Behav Res Ther ; 45(8): 1791-811, 2007 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17341415

ABSTRACT

To test cognitive models of panic disorder, a range of information processing biases were examined among persons with panic disorder (N=43) and healthy control participants (N=38). Evidence for automatic associations in memory was assessed using the Implicit Association Test, interference effects related to attention biases were assessed using a modified supraliminal Stroop task, and interpretation biases were assessed using the Brief Body Sensations Interpretation Questionnaire. In addition, the relationship between information processing biases and clinical markers of panic (including affective, behavioral, and cognitive symptom measures) was investigated, along with the relationships among biases. Results indicated more threat biases among the panic (relative to control) group on each of the information processing measures, providing some of the first evidence for an implicit measure of panic associations. Further, structural equation modeling indicated that the information processing bias measures were each unique predictors of panic symptoms, but that the bias indicators did not relate to one another. These findings suggest that cognitive factors may independently predict panic symptoms, but not covary. Results are discussed in terms of their support for cognitive models of panic and the potential for automatic versus strategic processing differences across the tasks to explain the low relationships across the biases.


Subject(s)
Cognition , Models, Psychological , Panic Disorder/psychology , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Association Learning , Attention , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Psychiatric Status Rating Scales , Psychometrics
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL