Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 5 de 5
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
J Sch Psychol ; 53(5): 375-91, 2015 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26407835

ABSTRACT

This study investigated the impact of victims' responses to overt bullying on peer bystanders' attitudes and reactions. Fifth- and seventh-grade students (N = 206; M(age) = 11.13 and 13.18 years, respectively) completed online questionnaires about gender-consistent videotaped hypothetical bullying scenarios in which the victims' responses (angry, sad, confident, ignoring) were experimentally manipulated. Victims' responses significantly influenced bystanders' attitudes towards the victim, perceptions of the victimization, emotional reactions, and behavioral intentions. In general, angry victims elicited more negative reactions, sad victims elicited greater intentions to act, while incidents involving confident victims were perceived as less serious. Several variations depending on the bullying type and students' grade, gender, and personal experiences with bullying were evident. Implications for individual-level and peer-level anti-bullying interventions are discussed.


Subject(s)
Bullying , Crime Victims/psychology , Emotions/physiology , Peer Group , Students/psychology , Adolescent , Adolescent Behavior/psychology , Child , Female , Humans , Intention , Male , Social Perception
2.
J Soc Psychol ; 154(6): 515-26, 2014.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25280167

ABSTRACT

We examined the impact of offset controllability (capability of losing weight) and offset effort (efforts to lose weight) on judgments of an obese target. Participants (n = 216) read about an obese person whose body weight was controllable/uncontrollable, and who did/did not put in effort to eat healthily and exercise. Effort played a more important role than controllability in evaluations of the target. Targets who put in effort to be healthy were ascribed fewer obesity stereotypes, evoked less disgust, and were considered to have a more acceptable lifestyle. These findings extend attribution theory and have implications for strategies to reduce weight bias.


Subject(s)
Attitude , Internal-External Control , Motivation , Obesity/psychology , Weight Loss , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Exercise/psychology , Feeding Behavior/psychology , Female , Humans , Judgment , Life Style , Male , Middle Aged , Social Desirability , Stereotyping , Young Adult
3.
PLoS One ; 8(11): e79268, 2013.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24236117

ABSTRACT

It is often assumed that social models influence people's eating behavior by providing a norm of appropriate food intake, but this hypothesis has not been directly tested. In three experiments, female participants were exposed to a low-intake model, a high-intake model, or no model (control condition). Experiments 1 and 2 used a remote-confederate manipulation and were conducted in the context of a cookie taste test. Experiment 3 used a live confederate and was conducted in the context of a task during which participants were given incidental access to food. Participants also rated the extent to which their food intake was influenced by a variety of factors (e.g., hunger, taste, how much others ate). In all three experiments, participants in the low-intake conditions ate less than did participants in the high-intake conditions, and also reported a lower perceived norm of appropriate intake. Furthermore, perceived norms of appropriate intake mediated the effects of the social model on participants' food intake. Despite the observed effects of the social models, participants were much more likely to indicate that their food intake was influenced by taste and hunger than by the behavior of the social models. Thus, social models appear to influence food intake by providing a norm of appropriate eating behavior, but people may be unaware of the influence of a social model on their behavior.


Subject(s)
Feeding Behavior , Models, Theoretical , Social Environment , Adolescent , Adult , Age Factors , Analysis of Variance , Female , Food Preferences , Humans , Risk Factors , Young Adult
5.
Behav Res Ther ; 50(5): 313-22, 2012 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22459730

ABSTRACT

Re-exposure to the unconditioned stimulus (US) following fear extinction in the laboratory produces reinstatement of fear. Similarly in clinical situations, anxiety patients may experience adverse events that reinstate fear following successful exposure therapy. The current study employed two USs, shock and loud noise, to examine whether a US that is qualitatively different but of the same valence as the original acquisition US can produce reinstatement in human fear conditioning. Both standard and cross-US reinstatement manipulations led to elevated fear as indexed by skin conductance. However, cross-US reinstatement was accompanied by elevated expectancy of the US that had been presented during the reinstatement manipulation, not the US that had been used to establish fear in acquisition. This result implies that reinstatement may involve the development of new fears. Context conditioning and cognitive processes were implicated as possible mechanisms. The current findings suggest that clinical relapse attributed to reinstatement may not always reflect the reactivation of old fears but may instead represent new fears worthy of clinical examination.


Subject(s)
Conditioning, Psychological/physiology , Extinction, Psychological/physiology , Fear/psychology , Galvanic Skin Response/physiology , Cues , Electroshock , Humans , Noise
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...