Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 9 de 9
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
J Child Lang ; 28(2): 373-91, 2001 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11449944

ABSTRACT

This study investigated the interface of form and function in the acquisition of negation in Cantonese-speaking children. The data, from the Hong Kong Cantonese Child Language Corpus, were longitudinal spontaneous samples of eight children aged 1;5 to 3;8. The main issues of the study were the sequence of emergence of negative markers mou5, m4 and mei6 and the acquisition trend of 11 semantic categories of negation in children's expressive language. The acquisition trend of the semantic categories matched Bloom's (1970, 1991a) finding that Non-existence preceded Rejection and Denial.


Subject(s)
Child Language , Language , Verbal Behavior , Age Factors , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Infant , Male , Semantics , Verbal Learning
2.
J Child Lang ; 28(1): 195-212, 2001 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11258004

ABSTRACT

Descriptions of the development of prosodic and segmental tiers of children's phonological systems have been derived from investigations of the development of English. This paper provides a preliminary description of phonological tier development in Cantonese-speaking children. Eight children, (two each at 1;7, 2;6, 3;5, and 4;2 years) named 95 pictures. The data were analysed for word, syllable, onset-rime, skeletal, and segmental tiers. The results suggested a developmental order in the acquisition of hierarchical features. Decreasing order of accuracy of the tiers was word = syllable > onset-rime = skeletal > segmental. A model of feature geometry was adopted to describe the acquisition of features. An interesting finding is the way the laryngeal feature (aspiration) was combined with place contrasts one at a time rather than all at once.


Subject(s)
Language , Speech , Verbal Behavior , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Infant , Male , Phonetics , Speech Production Measurement
3.
J Child Lang ; 28(3): 703-39, 2001 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11797545

ABSTRACT

The development of metalinguistic awareness, and specifically syntactic awareness, (here measured by age-related changes in the ability to judge and revise unacceptable sentences), reflects developmental changes in focus from semantic to syntactic properties of sentences. Previous research reported that children find judgements of word-order changes easier than morphological violations (Hakes, 1980). We hypothesized that this difference in ease of judgement is linked to the language under investigation. That is, there may be a relationship between the functional load of grammatical morphemes and ability to detect syntactic violations. This study investigated the development of syntactic awareness in Cantonese-speaking children. Fifty-six subjects from four age groups (three, five, seven and 20 years old) were asked to judge the grammaticality of 40 sentences (18 with word-order changes and 22 with morphological violations) and correct the grammatically deviant sentences. There was a significant age effect on subjects' performance in both judgement and revision tasks. Children scored significantly higher in judging sentences with word order changes than those with morphological violations. They also scored higher on word order revisions than morphological revisions, an unexpected finding. The success of correcting morphological violations varied by morphological marker, apparently according to each marker's degree of obligatory use in the language. It would seem then that syntactic awareness is very much affected by language-specific characteristics.


Subject(s)
Awareness , Child Language , Linguistics , Verbal Behavior , Adult , Age Factors , Child , Child, Preschool , Cognition , Female , Humans , Male
5.
Int J Lang Commun Disord ; 35(4): 527-41, 2000.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11091822

ABSTRACT

Recent studies of lexical diversity in English-speaking children with specific language impairment (ESLI) have produced conflicting results. Differences between SLI and age-matched (AM) groups on verb types, and overall types have been reported, as have differences between SLI and language-matched (LM) groups on verb types and/or verb tokens, and noun types and noun tokens. At the same time there have been other claims that there is no difference between SLI and LM groups in terms of lexical diversity. This comparison of the lexical diversity of Cantonese-speaking children with SLI (CSLI) and their language-matched peers controlled the length of the samples and the number of tokens as the basis for comparison. There was no difference between the groups in use of verb tokens or types, but there were significant differences in noun tokens and types and 'other' open class tokens and types. Although there was no difference between the groups in the use of a specific grammatical marker (aspect markers), the way in which the CSLI children deployed these markers was severely restricted in comparison with their LM peers. A limited capacity model of language production is invoked to explain the findings.


Subject(s)
Language Development Disorders/physiopathology , Vocabulary , Case-Control Studies , Child , Child, Preschool , China/ethnology , Hong Kong , Humans , Language Development Disorders/ethnology , Linguistics/methods
6.
Clin Linguist Phon ; 13(3): 183-97, 1999.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21275567

ABSTRACT

The substitution of [s] for word-initial aspirated targets was examined in a Cantonese-speaking pre-school child. Perceptual analysis showed that the child produced [ph] accurately, but substituted [s] for /t(h)/, /ts(h)/ and /k(h)/. Acoustic analysis revealed no subphonemic contrast between the target /s/, and the substituted [s] for /t(h)/, /ts(h)/ and /k(h)/. The unusual substitution of [s] for aspirated lingual targets, and the lack of a subphonemic contrast among [s] productions were described in terms of feature geometry. The target feature patterns of [-continuant, +spread] were produced as [+continuant, +spread], with maintenance of the default place feature (coronal). It is proposed that the child had immature motor control of the coordination of supralaryngeal and laryngeal movements. Examination of the child's productions of aspirated lingual targets at pre-, mid- and post-therapy revealed that compensatory articulation changed over time as the child learned to coordinate laryngeal and oral movements.

7.
Eur J Disord Commun ; 32(2 Spec No): 139-58, 1997.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9279431

ABSTRACT

The purpose of this investigation was to compare the ability of nurses and parents to detect speech and language disability. The methodology included development and validation of a parent questionnaire to detect speech and language disability in 398 3-year-old English-speaking children. The parent questionnaire and a standard developmental screen administered by child health nurses were compared with a gold standard (a speech and language assessment). International criteria for screening tools were applied. The tools were comparable in performance. A simple parent questionnaire, completed without professional assistance, was a viable alternative to professional screening. Parents' education and parity had no influence on their ability to detect language disorders. Although size of sibship had no association with the occurrence of speech and language disability, third-born children were more likely to have a speech and language disability than any other birth order.


Subject(s)
Language Development Disorders/prevention & control , Language Therapy/methods , Nurses , Parents , Child, Preschool , Female , Humans , Language Disorders/prevention & control , Male , Speech-Language Pathology
8.
Cleft Palate Craniofac J ; 33(2): 160-8, 1996 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8695625

ABSTRACT

This paper describes the use of electropalatography (EPG) in the treatment of a speech disorder in a Cantonese-speaking woman who had primary repair of the palate at age 13. A multiple-baseline approach was used to document treatment efficacy using electropalatography. The client showed rapid improvement in articulatory placement, with generalization to nontarget phonemes. In addition, improvement was noted in her manner of articulation, with a reduction of nasal emission. The relationship between articulatory placement errors and nasal emission in late repair cleft is discussed. Explanations for the effectiveness of EPG with this client are offered.


Subject(s)
Articulation Disorders/therapy , Biofeedback, Psychology/instrumentation , Cleft Palate/complications , Speech Therapy/methods , Adolescent , Articulation Disorders/etiology , Articulation Disorders/physiopathology , China , Cleft Palate/surgery , Female , Humans , Palate/physiopathology , Phonetics , Speech Production Measurement , Speech Therapy/instrumentation , Tongue/physiopathology , Voice Quality
9.
Eur J Disord Commun ; 31(1): 45-64, 1996.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8776431

ABSTRACT

The subjects were seven Cantonese-speaking children with repaired cleft palate, aged between 3;2 and 10;2. Speech samples were obtained by use of two word lists. Error patterns were described by use of a detailed analysis based on perceptual judgements. A 'confusion matrix' was constructed for each subject to illustrate the relative frequency of error type. The results showed high within-subject and across-subject variability. The data partially support findings from other languages, for example, showing posterior placement for alveolar targets. However, some language-specific errors were identified, such as initial consonant deletion. Four possible explanations for the data are discussed.


Subject(s)
Articulation Disorders/etiology , Cleft Palate/complications , Child , Child, Preschool , Humans , Language , Phonetics
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...