Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 9 de 9
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Brain Struct Funct ; 221(2): 979-95, 2016 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25432772

ABSTRACT

The current study provides a generalizable account of the anatomo-functional associations as well as the connectivity of representational codes underlying numerical processing as suggested by the triple code model (TCM) of numerical cognition. By evaluating the neural networks subserving numerical cognition in two specific and substantially different numerical tasks with regard to both grey matter localizations as well as white matter tracts we (1) considered the possibility of additional memory-related cortex areas crucial for arithmetic fact retrieval (e.g., the hippocampus); (2) specified the functional involvement of prefrontal areas in number magnitude processing, and, finally; (3) identified the connections between these anatomo-functional instantiations of the representations involved in number magnitude processing and arithmetic fact retrieval employing probabilistic fiber tracking. The resulting amendments to the TCM are summarized in a schematic update, and ideas concerning the possible functional interplay between number magnitude processing and arithmetic fact retrieval are discussed.


Subject(s)
Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Cognition/physiology , Cerebral Cortex/anatomy & histology , Cerebral Cortex/diagnostic imaging , Connectome , Diffusion Tensor Imaging/methods , Humans , Memory/physiology , White Matter/metabolism
2.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 35(3): 1031-43, 2014 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23283834

ABSTRACT

By implementing a task that closely resembled a clinical test for diagnosing spatial neglect in stroke patients, Himmelbach et al. (: Neuroimage 32:1747-1759) found significantly increased activation during active exploration in those cortical areas in healthy subjects that are known to induce spatial neglect in case of a lesion. The present study investigated whether direct intra-hemispheric cortico-cortical connections could be found between these activated clusters using a probabilistic fiber-tracking approach in 52 healthy subjects. We found that parts of the extreme capsule (EmC) and the middle longitudinal fascicle (MdLF) connected the functional cluster in the prefrontal cortex with the superior temporal cortex and the temporo-parietal junction (TPJ) area in both hemispheres. The activation peak in the TPJ was additionally connected to the inferior frontal cortex by parts of the arcuate fascicle and the superior longitudinal fascicle (SLF II) in the right hemisphere. Our study elucidates the connections constituting the perisylvian network for spatial orienting and attention. Hence, we complement the knowledge from patients suffering from spatial neglect by giving first empirical evidence for the complete postulated network in healthy subjects.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Cerebral Cortex/physiology , Diffusion Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Neural Pathways/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Adult , Diffusion Magnetic Resonance Imaging/instrumentation , Diffusion Tensor Imaging/instrumentation , Diffusion Tensor Imaging/methods , Exploratory Behavior/physiology , Female , Functional Laterality , Humans , Male , Nerve Fibers/physiology , Orientation/physiology , Spatial Behavior/physiology , Young Adult
3.
Neurocase ; 20(2): 158-62, 2014 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23286466

ABSTRACT

Symptoms of limb apraxia and executive dysfunctions are currently not explicitly considered by the National Institutes of Health Stroke Scale and, thus, not routinely tested by clinicians in the acute care of patients with suspected stroke. Neuropsychological testing, clinical examination, MRI, and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) were performed in a right-handed patient with acute onset of left-sided sensorimotor hemiparesis due to a right hemisphere ischemic stroke. Deficits in the execution of meaningless and meaningful gestures were not detected properly on initial clinical examination but were revealed later on through neuropsychological testing. Instead, the patient's inability to respond to specific instructions in the acute care setting was mistaken to reflect severe deficits in auditory comprehension. fMRI revealed right-hemispheric localization of language in the right-handed patient. We suggest including a bedside test for limb apraxia symptoms in acute clinical care of stroke patients. The distinction between deficits in limb praxis and impairments of language can be complicated owing to the common hemispheric co-localization of the two functions.


Subject(s)
Apraxias/diagnosis , Stroke/complications , Apraxias/etiology , Brain/pathology , Brain/physiopathology , Brain Ischemia/complications , Brain Mapping , Extremities/physiopathology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests
4.
Brain ; 136(Pt 2): 619-29, 2013 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23378217

ABSTRACT

Converging evidence from neuroimaging studies and computational modelling suggests an organization of language in a dual dorsal-ventral brain network: a dorsal stream connects temporoparietal with frontal premotor regions through the superior longitudinal and arcuate fasciculus and integrates sensorimotor processing, e.g. in repetition of speech. A ventral stream connects temporal and prefrontal regions via the extreme capsule and mediates meaning, e.g. in auditory comprehension. The aim of our study was to test, in a large sample of 100 aphasic stroke patients, how well acute impairments of repetition and comprehension correlate with lesions of either the dorsal or ventral stream. We combined voxelwise lesion-behaviour mapping with the dorsal and ventral white matter fibre tracts determined by probabilistic fibre tracking in our previous study in healthy subjects. We found that repetition impairments were mainly associated with lesions located in the posterior temporoparietal region with a statistical lesion maximum in the periventricular white matter in projection of the dorsal superior longitudinal and arcuate fasciculus. In contrast, lesions associated with comprehension deficits were found more ventral-anterior in the temporoprefrontal region with a statistical lesion maximum between the insular cortex and the putamen in projection of the ventral extreme capsule. Individual lesion overlap with the dorsal fibre tract showed a significant negative correlation with repetition performance, whereas lesion overlap with the ventral fibre tract revealed a significant negative correlation with comprehension performance. To summarize, our results from patients with acute stroke lesions support the claim that language is organized along two segregated dorsal-ventral streams. Particularly, this is the first lesion study demonstrating that task performance on auditory comprehension measures requires an interaction between temporal and prefrontal brain regions via the ventral extreme capsule pathway.


Subject(s)
Aphasia/pathology , Language , Nerve Net/pathology , Prefrontal Cortex/pathology , Temporal Lobe/pathology , Acute Disease , Adolescent , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Aphasia/physiopathology , Auditory Perception/physiology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Nerve Net/physiology , Neural Pathways/pathology , Neural Pathways/physiology , Prefrontal Cortex/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Temporal Lobe/physiology , Young Adult
5.
Neurology ; 78(10): 728-35, 2012 Mar 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22357719

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: Perception of verticality can be perturbed after cortical stroke. However, a relationship between lesion location and pathologic perception of verticality is still a matter of debate since previous studies revealed contradictory results. Thus, the aim of the current study was to test whether specific cortical lesions were associated with tilts of subjective visual vertical (SVV) and to determine the critical brain areas that cause such tilts in the case of a lesion. METHODS: SVV was systematically studied in 54 patients (22 patients with left-sided and 32 patients with right-sided lesions) with acute unilateral strokes, analyzed by modern voxel-wise lesion-behavior mapping techniques. RESULTS: The data give evidence for an association between tilt of SVV and the insular cortex (IC) and inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) in both hemispheres. Whereas the IC seems to be the prominent structure in the left hemisphere, the IFG is most affected in the right hemisphere. Furthermore, other cortical regions such as the superior temporal gyrus (STG) and the rolandic operculum as well as-subcortically-the inferior occipitofrontal fascicle and the superior longitudinal fascicle seem to be involved in the vestibulo-cortical network for the perception of verticality in the roll plane. CONCLUSION: Damage to these regions might lead to an imbalance within the vestibular network of one hemisphere due to a deficit in multimodal signal processing.


Subject(s)
Brain/physiopathology , Perceptual Disorders/physiopathology , Space Perception/physiology , Stroke/physiopathology , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Brain Mapping , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Perceptual Disorders/etiology , Stroke/complications
6.
Neuropsychologia ; 50(6): 1136-41, 2012 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22230231

ABSTRACT

While unilateral spatial neglect after left brain damage is undoubtedly less common than spatial neglect after a right hemisphere lesion, it is also assumed to be less severe. Here we directly test this latter hypothesis using a continuous measure of neglect severity: the so-called Center of Cancellation (CoC). Rorden and Karnath (2010) recently validated this index for right brain damaged neglect patients. A first aim of the present study was to evaluate this new measure for spatial neglect after left brain damage. In a group of 48 left-sided stroke patients with and without neglect, a score greater than -0.086 on the Bells Test and greater than -0.024 on the Letter Cancellation Task turned out to indicate neglect behavior for acute left brain damaged patients. A second aim was to directly compare the severity of spatial neglect after left versus right brain injury by using the new CoC measure. While neglect is less frequent following left than right hemisphere injury, we found that when this symptom occurs it is of similar severity in acute left brain injury as in patients after acute right brain injury.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Brain Injuries/complications , Functional Laterality/physiology , Perceptual Disorders/etiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Female , Humans , Linear Models , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Perceptual Disorders/diagnosis , Severity of Illness Index , Young Adult
7.
Brain ; 134(Pt 10): 3059-70, 2011 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21624927

ABSTRACT

During evolution, the human brain developed remarkable functional differences between left and right hemispheres. Due to this lateralization, disorders of spatial orienting occur predominantly after right brain damage and disorders of language after left brain damage. In contrast to this general pattern, few individuals show disturbed spatial orienting (spatial neglect) after left brain damage. Using a voxel-based lesion analysis approach, we found that neglect after acute left brain damage is represented in areas typically serving language functions, namely the superior and middle temporal gyri, inferior parietal lobule and insula. Since all except one of these patients also suffered from aphasia, we conclude that lateralization is not just reversed but that both functions (language and spatial orienting) rather are represented in the same left hemisphere regions. We speculate that a representation of spatial orienting in left hemisphere language areas might be a phylogenetic relict in humans, though this representation stays subdominant in the vast majority of individuals.


Subject(s)
Aphasia/physiopathology , Brain/physiopathology , Functional Laterality/physiology , Perceptual Disorders/physiopathology , Space Perception/physiology , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Brain Mapping , Female , Humans , Language , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests
8.
Neuropsychologia ; 49(5): 1294-1301, 2011 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21236271

ABSTRACT

It is widely accepted that letter-by-letter reading and a pronounced increase in reading time as a function of word length are the hallmark features of pure alexia. Why patients show these two phenomena with respect to underlying cognitive mechanisms is, however, much less clear. Two main hypotheses have been proposed, i.e. impaired discrimination of letters and deficient processing of word forms. While the former deficit can easily be investigated in isolation, previous findings favouring the latter seem confounded. Applying a word reading paradigm with systematically manipulated letter orders in two patients with pure alexia, we demonstrate a word form processing deficit that is not attributable to sublexical letter discrimination difficulties. Moreover, pure alexia-like fixation patterns could be induced in healthy adults by having them read sentences including words with transposed letters, so-called 'jumbled words'. This further corroborates a key role of deficient word form processing in pure alexia. With regard to basic reading research, the present study extends recent evidence for relative, rather than precise, encoding of letter position in the brain.


Subject(s)
Alexia, Pure/complications , Comprehension/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Reading , Vocabulary , Aged , Alexia, Pure/pathology , Brain/pathology , Humans , Magnetic Resonance Imaging/methods , Male , Middle Aged , Reaction Time/physiology
9.
Perception ; 38(4): 522-32, 2009.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19522321

ABSTRACT

Biological-motion perception consists of a number of different phenomena. They include global mechanisms that support the retrieval of the coherent shape of a walker, but also mechanisms which derive information from the local motion of its parts about facing direction and animacy, independent of the particular shape of the display. A large body of the literature on biological-motion perception is based on a synthetic stimulus generated by an algorithm published by James Cutting in 1978 (Perception 7 393-405). Here we show that this particular stimulus lacks a visual invariant inherent to the local motion of the feet of a natural walker, which in more realistic motion patterns indicates the facing direction of a walker independent of its shape. Comparing Cutting's walker to a walker derived from motion-captured data of real human walkers, we find no difference between the two displays in a detection task designed such that observers had to rely on global shape. In a direction discrimination task, however, in which only local motion was accessible to the observer, performance on Cutting's walker was at chance, while direction could still be retrieved from the stimuli derived from the real walker.


Subject(s)
Discrimination, Psychological , Motion Perception/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Walking , Adolescent , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Biomechanical Phenomena/physiology , Female , Foot , Humans , Male , Photic Stimulation/methods , Young Adult
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...