Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 3 de 3
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Neuropsychologia ; 99: 207-212, 2017 05.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28322905

ABSTRACT

Bilingualism has been found to delay onset of dementia and this has been attributed to an advantage in executive control in bilinguals. However, the relationship between bilingualism and cognition is complex, with costs as well as benefits to language functions. To further explore the cognitive consequences of bilingualism, the study used Frontotemporal dementia (FTD) syndromes, to examine whether bilingualism modifies the age at onset of behavioral and language variants of Frontotemporal dementia (FTD) differently. Case records of 193 patients presenting with FTD (121 of them bilingual) were examined and the age at onset of the first symptoms were compared between monolinguals and bilinguals. A significant effect of bilingualism delaying the age at onset of dementia was found in behavioral variant FTD (5.7 years) but not in progressive nonfluent aphasia (0.7 years), semantic dementia (0.5 years), corticobasal syndrome (0.4 years), progressive supranuclear palsy (4.3 years) and FTD-motor neuron disease (3 years). On dividing all patients predominantly behavioral and predominantly aphasic groups, age at onset in the bilingual behavioral group (62.6) was over 6 years higher than in the monolingual patients (56.5, p=0.006), while there was no difference in the aphasic FTD group (60.9 vs. 60.6 years, p=0.851). The bilingual effect on age of bvFTD onset was shown independently of other potential confounding factors such as education, gender, occupation, and urban vs rural dwelling of subjects. To conclude, bilingualism delays the age at onset in the behavioral but not in the aphasic variants of FTD. The results are in line with similar findings based on research in stroke and with the current views of the interaction between bilingualism and cognition, pointing to advantages in executive functions and disadvantages in lexical tasks.


Subject(s)
Frontotemporal Dementia/epidemiology , Frontotemporal Dementia/psychology , Multilingualism , Adult , Age of Onset , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Aphasia/classification , Aphasia/epidemiology , Female , Frontotemporal Dementia/classification , Humans , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Middle Aged , Registries , Time Factors
2.
Stroke ; 47(1): 258-61, 2016 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26585392

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND AND PURPOSE: Bilingualism has been associated with slower cognitive aging and a later onset of dementia. In this study, we aimed to determine whether bilingualism also influences cognitive outcome after stroke. METHODS: We examined 608 patients with ischemic stroke from a large stroke registry and studied the role of bilingualism in predicting poststroke cognitive impairment in the absence of dementia. RESULTS: A larger proportion of bilinguals had normal cognition compared with monolinguals (40.5% versus 19.6%; P<0.0001), whereas the reverse was noted in patients with cognitive impairment, including vascular dementia and vascular mild cognitive impairment (monolinguals 77.7% versus bilinguals 49.0%; P<0.0009). There were no differences in the frequency of aphasia (monolinguals 11.8% versus bilinguals 10.5%; P=0.354). Bilingualism was found to be an independent predictor of poststroke cognitive impairment. CONCLUSIONS: Our results suggest that bilingualism leads to a better cognitive outcome after stroke, possibly by enhancing cognitive reserve.


Subject(s)
Cognitive Dysfunction/diagnosis , Cognitive Dysfunction/psychology , Cognitive Reserve , Multilingualism , Stroke/diagnosis , Stroke/psychology , Adult , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Cognitive Dysfunction/epidemiology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Registries , Stroke/epidemiology
3.
Neurology ; 81(22): 1938-44, 2013 Nov 26.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24198291

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: The purpose of the study was to determine the association between bilingualism and age at onset of dementia and its subtypes, taking into account potential confounding factors. METHODS: Case records of 648 patients with dementia (391 of them bilingual) diagnosed in a specialist clinic were reviewed. The age at onset of first symptoms was compared between monolingual and bilingual groups. The influence of number of languages spoken, education, occupation, and other potentially interacting variables was examined. RESULTS: Overall, bilingual patients developed dementia 4.5 years later than the monolingual ones. A significant difference in age at onset was found across Alzheimer disease dementia as well as frontotemporal dementia and vascular dementia, and was also observed in illiterate patients. There was no additional benefit to speaking more than 2 languages. The bilingual effect on age at dementia onset was shown independently of other potential confounding factors such as education, sex, occupation, and urban vs rural dwelling of subjects. CONCLUSIONS: This is the largest study so far documenting a delayed onset of dementia in bilingual patients and the first one to show it separately in different dementia subtypes. It is the first study reporting a bilingual advantage in those who are illiterate, suggesting that education is not a sufficient explanation for the observed difference. The findings are interpreted in the context of the bilingual advantages in attention and executive functions.


Subject(s)
Dementia/epidemiology , Dementia/psychology , Educational Status , Emigration and Immigration , Multilingualism , Adult , Age of Onset , Aged , Aged, 80 and over , Dementia/physiopathology , Female , Humans , Linear Models , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Middle Aged
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...