Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 16 de 16
Filter
1.
Psychiatry Res ; 273: 487-492, 2019 03.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30685731

ABSTRACT

The aim of this study was to explore sleep quality in a video game population and to prospect the role of different factors such as sociodemographic data, video game duration, intensity of video game playing, and mental and physical health. Two hundred and seventeen participants (24.40 ±â€¯6.98 years old) completed an online questionnaire composed of sociodemographic informations, the Pittsburg Sleep Quality Index (PSQI), the Medical Outcomes Study (MOS SF-36), video game play time per week, and intensity of video game playing as defined by Décamps (AIE-Q). We carried out hierarchical cluster analysis on the 7 dimensions of PSQI to determine sleep quality profiles. Two profiles were found: (1) « High sleep quality profile ¼ for 132 (60.83%) participants, and (2) « Low sleep quality profile ¼ for 85 (39.17%) participants. These two profiles were differently associated with video game duration per week, intensity of video game playing, and mental health. Sleep quality was positively related to mental health and negatively with intensity of video game playing. Intensity of video game playing was a more salient factor to predict poor sleep quality than video game duration.


Subject(s)
Mental Disorders/physiopathology , Sleep Initiation and Maintenance Disorders/psychology , Sleep , Video Games/psychology , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Mental Disorders/psychology , Mental Health , Surveys and Questionnaires , Young Adult
2.
Behav Sleep Med ; 17(6): 729-739, 2019.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29923749

ABSTRACT

Background: Many adolescents and young adults get insufficient sleep. A link between sleep loss and risk-taking behavior has been consistently found in the literature, but surprisingly, the role played by the origin of sleep loss in this link has never been investigated. Sleep loss can be voluntary (instead of sleeping, a significant amount of time is devoted to other activities) or involuntary (caused by a sleep disorder, for example, insomnia). The aim of this research was to investigate whether both types of sleep loss are associated to the same extent with risky behavior. Participants: Five hundred thirty-six university students between 19 and 25 years old participated in this study. Three groups were selected: participants with voluntary sleep loss, participants with insomnia, and normal sleepers. Methods: We assessed risk-taking behavior in virtual driving situations, as well as drinking habits in terms of quantity and frequency. To further explore the differences between the groups, we also measured sensation seeking, a personality trait related to risk-taking behavior. Results: Compared to participants with insomnia and normal sleepers, participants with voluntary sleep loss take more risks in dangerous driving situations, drink more alcohol, and have higher disinhibition scores on the Sensation-Seeking Scale. On the other hand, no such differences were found between participants with insomnia and normal sleepers, suggesting that sleep loss is not always associated with risk taking. Conclusions: Whether sleep loss is associated with risk-taking behavior or not could depend on the origin of sleep loss and the underlying personality traits.


Subject(s)
Sleep Initiation and Maintenance Disorders/psychology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Risk-Taking , Surveys and Questionnaires , Young Adult
3.
Chronobiol Int ; 33(7): 937-41, 2016.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27101348

ABSTRACT

Inclination toward eveningness is often associated with risky behavior. But the existing studies are scarce, inconsistent and usually limited to self-reported measures. We sought to investigate in young adults whether morningness-eveningness is associated with risky behavior in dangerous driving situations, with self-reported drunk driving and with alcohol consumption. Results show that, indeed, inclination toward eveningness is associated with these risky behaviors. We also demonstrate a link between morningness-eveningness and sensation seeking. Therefore, young adults with a tendency toward eveningness might be more at risk to face negative consequences of alcohol abuse or to be involved in a road accident.


Subject(s)
Alcohol Drinking , Automobile Driving , Circadian Rhythm/physiology , Personality/physiology , Risk-Taking , Accidents, Traffic/statistics & numerical data , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Risk , Self Report , Young Adult
4.
Am J Health Behav ; 39(5): 623-31, 2015 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26248172

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVES: Our study aimed to explore how neuroticism and neuroticism-related traits as well as sleep-related cognitions (dysfunctional beliefs and subjective quality of sleep) influence the emergence of insomnia using a mediational model. METHODS: A cross-sectional study was conducted in which 159 insomniac patients paired with 159 normal sleepers in sex and age (N = 318) completed an online questionnaire. RESULTS: At the global level, dysfunctional beliefs and poor subjective quality of sleep mediated the neuroticism-insomnia path; at the trait-specific level, these variables mediated the anxiety-insomnia path and partially mediated the effects of vulnerability and self-consciousness on insomnia; some other relations were essentially indirect effects (between depression and insomnia). CONCLUSIONS: These findings extend our understanding of how neuroticism is a predisposing factor of insomnia. This knowledge could be helpful to shape prevention and intervention programs to treat insomnia.


Subject(s)
Anxiety Disorders/psychology , Cognition , Sleep Initiation and Maintenance Disorders/psychology , Sleep , Adult , Aged , Anxiety Disorders/complications , Case-Control Studies , Cross-Sectional Studies , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Neuroticism , Sleep Initiation and Maintenance Disorders/complications , Young Adult
5.
Sleep Med ; 14(10): 964-72, 2013 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23948221

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Few studies have examined the impact of continuous positive airway pressure (CPAP) therapy on short-term memory (STM) over sustained wakefulness in patients with obstructive sleep apnea-hypopnea syndrome (OSAHS). We have investigated if impaired STM can be reversed by CPAP treatment in a 24-h sustained wakefulness paradigm. METHODS: Our follow-up study was conducted with repeated-memory tasks within 12 OSAHS patients and 10 healthy controls who underwent three 32-h sessions, one before CPAP (T0) and the second (T3) and the third (T6), after 3 and 6 months of treatment, respectively, for OSAHS patients. Each session included one night of sleep followed by 24h of sustained wakefulness, during which both groups performed STM tasks including both digit span (DS) and Sternberg tasks. RESULTS: Untreated OSAHS patients had no deficit in the forward DS task measuring immediate memory but were impaired in STM, especially working memory assessed by the complex Sternberg task and the backward DS. However, only performance in the latter was improved after 6 months of CPAP treatment. CONCLUSIONS: Because the high level of memory scanning required high speed in information processing, persistent impairment on the complex Sternberg task may be attributable to working memory slowing, possibly enhanced by sustained wakefulness.


Subject(s)
Continuous Positive Airway Pressure/methods , Memory, Short-Term/physiology , Sleep Apnea, Obstructive/physiopathology , Sleep Apnea, Obstructive/therapy , Wakefulness/physiology , Adult , Attention/physiology , Circadian Rhythm/physiology , Cognition/physiology , Disorders of Excessive Somnolence/physiopathology , Disorders of Excessive Somnolence/therapy , Female , Follow-Up Studies , Humans , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Polysomnography , Sleep Stages/physiology
6.
Sleep ; 35(7): 997-1002, 2012 Jul 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22754046

ABSTRACT

STUDY OBJECTIVE: To evaluate the effects of acute sleep deprivation and chronic sleep restriction on vigilance, performance, and self-perception of sleepiness. DESIGN: Habitual night followed by 1 night of total sleep loss (acute sleep deprivation) or 5 consecutive nights of 4 hr of sleep (chronic sleep restriction) and recovery night. PARTICIPANTS: Eighteen healthy middle-aged male participants (age [(± standard deviation] = 49.7 ± 2.6 yr, range 46-55 yr). MEASUREMENTS: Multiple sleep latency test trials, Karolinska Sleepiness Scale scores, simple reaction time test (lapses and 10% fastest reaction times), and nocturnal polysomnography data were recorded. RESULTS: Objective and subjective sleepiness increased immediately in response to sleep restriction. Sleep latencies after the second and third nights of sleep restriction reached levels equivalent to those observed after acute sleep deprivation, whereas Karolinska Sleepiness Scale scores did not reach these levels. Lapse occurrence increased after the second day of sleep restriction and reached levels equivalent to those observed after acute sleep deprivation. A statistical model revealed that sleepiness and lapses did not progressively worsen across days of sleep restriction. Ten percent fastest reaction times (i.e., optimal alertness) were not affected by acute or chronic sleep deprivation. Recovery to baseline levels of alertness and performance occurred after 8-hr recovery night. CONCLUSIONS: In middle-aged study participants, sleep restriction induced a high increase in sleep propensity but adaptation to chronic sleep restriction occurred beyond day 3 of restriction. This sleepiness attenuation was underestimated by the participants. One recovery night restores daytime sleepiness and cognitive performance deficits induced by acute or chronic sleep deprivation. CITATION: Philip P; Sagaspe P; Prague M; Tassi P; Capelli A; Bioulac B; Commenges D; Taillard J. Acute versus chronic partial sleep deprivation in middle-aged people: differential effect on performance and sleepiness. SLEEP 2012;35(7):997-1002.


Subject(s)
Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Sleep Deprivation/physiopathology , Wakefulness/physiology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Polysomnography , Reaction Time/physiology , Sleep/physiology , Sleep Deprivation/psychology
7.
Sleep Med ; 13(1): 29-35, 2012 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22177345

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Respiratory events during sleep usually lead to micro arousals resulting in consecutive daytime sleepiness even in healthy snorers. The present study investigated the evolution of subjective and objective daytime sleepiness and reaction time in healthy snorers submitted to acute and chronic sleep deprivation. METHODS: Objective sleepiness was measured by the MSLT, subjective sleepiness by the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS), and reaction time (RT) by the Psychomotor Vigilance Test. Mean sleep latencies, KSS scores and performance were analyzed through repeated measures ANOVAs with one between-factor (snorers and non-snorers) and two within-factors (sleep deprivation [baseline, acute, and chronic sleep deprivation] and time-of-day). RESULTS: The findings reveal that sleep deprivation does not enhance snoring but that, during baseline, objective daytime sleepiness is higher in snorers than in non-snorers (shorter sleep latencies) with no difference in subjective assessments. The effects of acute and chronic sleep deprivation on sleep are similar in both groups, but, after acute sleep deprivation, RT and attentional lapses (RT >500 ms) are higher in snorers. Chronic sleep deprivation produces similar results in both groups. CONCLUSION: These results suggest that respiratory efforts may be involved in the increased vulnerability to sleep deprivation of healthy snorers when compared to non-snorers.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Psychomotor Performance/physiology , Sleep Deprivation/psychology , Snoring/psychology , Adult , Arousal/physiology , Case-Control Studies , Cognition/physiology , Humans , Male , Polysomnography , Reaction Time/physiology , Sleep Deprivation/physiopathology , Snoring/physiopathology , Young Adult
8.
Brain Cogn ; 75(1): 39-50, 2011 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21041012

ABSTRACT

Both working and immediate memories were assessed every 4h by specific short-term memory tasks over sustained wakefulness in 12 patients with obstructive sleep apnea and hypopnea syndrome (OSAHS) and 10 healthy controls. Results indicated that OSAHS patients exhibited lower working memory performances than controls on both backward digit span and complex Sternberg tasks. Speed and accuracy on Sternberg tasks were affected by memory load in both groups. However, immediate memory was not impaired in OSAHS patients. Diurnal and nocturnal SaO(2) were correlated with speed and accuracy high-speed memory scanning performance on Sternberg tasks in patients. These results suggest specific working memory deficits associated with OSAHS over sustained wakefulness with a possible deficiency in the central executive responsible for the higher information processing, in addition to a potentially insufficient storage capacity. Among OSAHS patients, working memory ability involved in high-speed memory scanning may be impaired by chronic hypoxemia.


Subject(s)
Executive Function , Hypoxia/psychology , Memory, Short-Term , Oxygen/blood , Sleep Apnea, Obstructive/psychology , Wakefulness , Case-Control Studies , Female , Humans , Hypoxia/complications , Hypoxia/etiology , Male , Middle Aged , Neuropsychological Tests , Polysomnography , Severity of Illness Index , Sleep Apnea, Obstructive/blood , Sleep Apnea, Obstructive/complications
9.
Environ Int ; 36(7): 683-9, 2010 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20569986

ABSTRACT

Very few studies were devoted to permanent effects of nocturnal railway noise on sleep and cardiovascular reactivity. We investigated the effects of nocturnal railway noise on sleep and cardiovascular response in young and middle-aged adults living for many years either near a railway track or in a quiet area. Forty subjects (50% males) divided into two age groups (juniors: 26.2+/-3.6 and seniors: 56.2+/-4.2) participated in this experiment. Half of them lived near a railway track (RW group: 2.6 to 19 years) and the other half in a quiet environment (QE group: 8.1 to 14.2 years). After an adaptation night, all subjects underwent two nights in the laboratory: one control night and one noisy night (30 by-passes of a freight train). Sleep and cardiovascular modifications were assessed in response to noise. Sleep fragmentation indices were lower in RW subjects compared to QE whatever their age. In response to noise, there was a higher cardiovascular response rate to noise in RW juniors and a lower cardiovascular response rate in RW seniors compared to their age-paired QE counterparts. In conclusion, permanent exposure to nocturnal railway noise leads to decreased sleep fragmentation and to cardiovascular habituation. It is suggested that during the initial period experienced by residents living near railway tracks, nocturnal railway noise could induce a sensitization process on the autonomic response to noise reflecting a startle/defense reflex due to its functional significance, which progressively turns to habituation in the long-term if no adverse effect is experienced.


Subject(s)
Environmental Exposure/analysis , Environmental Monitoring , Heart Rate/physiology , Noise, Transportation/statistics & numerical data , Railroads , Sleep Deprivation/epidemiology , Adult , Age Factors , Environmental Exposure/statistics & numerical data , Epidemiological Monitoring , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Residence Characteristics , Time , Young Adult
10.
Eur J Appl Physiol ; 108(4): 671-80, 2010 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19902241

ABSTRACT

The aim of this study was to investigate the effects of nocturnal railway noise on cardiovascular reactivity in young (25.8 +/- 2.6 years) and middle-aged (52.2 +/- 2.5 years) adults during sleep. Thirty-eight subjects slept three nights in the laboratory at 1-week interval. They were exposed to 48 randomized pass-bys of Freight, Passenger and Automotive trains either at an 8-h equivalent sound level of 40 dBA (Moderate) and 50 dBA (High) or at a silent Control night. Heart rate response (HRR), heart response amplitude (HRA), heart response latency (HRL) and finger pulse response (FPR), finger pulse amplitude (FPA) and finger pulse latency (FPL) were recorded to measure cardiovascular reactivity after each noise onset and for time-matched pseudo-noises in the control condition. Results show that Freight trains produced the highest cardiac response (increased HRR, HRA and HRL) compared to Passenger and Automotive. But the vascular response was similar whatever the type of train. Juniors exhibited an increased HRR and HRA as compared to seniors, but there was no age difference on vasoconstriction, except a shorter FPL in seniors. Noise level produced dose-dependent effects on all the cardiovascular indices. Sleep stage at noise occurrence was ineffective for cardiac response, but FPA was reduced when noise occurred during REM sleep. In conclusion, our study is in favor of an important impact of nocturnal railway noise on the cardiovascular system of sleeping subjects. In the limit of the samples studied, Freight trains are the most harmful, probably more because of their special length (duration) than because of their speed (rise time).


Subject(s)
Cardiovascular Physiological Phenomena , Noise, Transportation , Railroads , Sleep/physiology , Adult , Age Factors , Female , Heart Rate/physiology , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Noise, Transportation/adverse effects , Polysomnography , Vasoconstriction/physiology , Young Adult
11.
Int J Occup Saf Ergon ; 14(4): 387-94, 2008.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19080043

ABSTRACT

The study was conducted to analyse possible interactions between noise and shift work, and fatigue as a function of age. In a large questionnaire survey, we assessed personal and environmental risk factors related to fatigue. Noise exposure at work (L(Aeq, 8hr)) was measured with personal noise dosimetry. The sample included 254 day and shift workers, and was divided into 2 age groups (<40- and >40-year-olds). Noise exposure had a main effect on fatigue. The highest noise exposure resulted in an increase in the fatigue level of older shift workers. The quantity of sleep mainly depended on the type of shift and age. Our data suggest that the most important factor generating fatigue could be related to industrial noise exposure, a factor which seems to aggravate work-related fatigue generated in a synergic manner by shift work and ageing. Senior workers should be prevented from cumulating those occupational stressors.


Subject(s)
Fatigue/etiology , Noise, Occupational/adverse effects , Work Schedule Tolerance/physiology , Adult , Age Factors , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Surveys and Questionnaires , Young Adult
12.
Int J Psychophysiol ; 70(3): 184-91, 2008 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18773929

ABSTRACT

Due to undisputable effects of noise on sleep structure, especially in terms of sleep fragmentation, the expected development of railway transportation in the next few years might represent a potential risk factor for people living alongside the rail tracks. The aim of this study was to compare the effects of different types of train (freight, automotive, passenger) on arousal from sleep and to determine any differential impact as a function of sound level and age. Twenty young (16 women, 4 men; 25.8 years+/-2.6) and 18 middle-aged (15 women, 3 men; 52.2 years+/-2.5) healthy subjects participated in three whole-night polysomnographic recordings including one control night (35 dBA), and two noisy nights with equivalent noise levels of 40 or 50 dB(A), respectively. Arousal responsiveness increased with sound level. It was the highest in S2 and the lowest in REM sleep. Micro-arousals (3-10 s) occurred at a rate of 25-30%, irrespective of the type of train. Awakenings (>10 s) were produced more frequently by freight train than by automotive and passenger trains. Normal age-related changes in sleep were observed, but they were not aggravated by railway noise, thus questioning whether older persons are less sensitive to noise during sleep. These evidences led to the conclusion that microscopic detection of sleep fragmentation may provide advantageous information on sleep disturbances caused by environmental noises.


Subject(s)
Arousal/physiology , Noise, Transportation/adverse effects , Sleep Initiation and Maintenance Disorders/etiology , Sleep Stages/physiology , Adult , Age Factors , Analysis of Variance , Fatigue/etiology , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Reaction Time/physiology , Sleep Initiation and Maintenance Disorders/complications , Sleep Initiation and Maintenance Disorders/psychology , Young Adult
13.
Physiol Behav ; 87(1): 177-84, 2006 Jan 30.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16303153

ABSTRACT

Sleep inertia (SI) is a transient period occurring immediately after awakening, usually characterized by performance decrement. When sleep is sufficient, SI is moderate, and produces few or no deficit. When it is associated with prior sleep deprivation, SI shows dose-dependent negative effects on cognitive performance, especially when subjects have been awaken in slow wave sleep (SWS). In the present study, spectral analysis was applied during the last 10 min before and the first 10 min after awakening, and during 1 h after awakening while subjects performed the Stroop test. Seventeen subjects were divided into a Control group who slept 8 h, and a Sleep Deprived group who slept only 2 h. The results show that performance was normal in the Control group, whereas reaction time was increased during the first half hour and error level during the second half hour in the Sleep Deprived group. Spectral analysis applied on the waking EEG during the whole test session showed that alpha activity was increased in both groups, but theta power only in the Sleep Deprived group. There was a high positive correlation in sleep deprived subjects between delta power during the last 10 min of sleep and subsequent performance decrement in speed and accuracy. Comparison of individual records showed a high positive correlation between spectral power before and after awakening in the Control group (generally in the sense of an increased frequency band), but no correlation was found in the Sleep Deprived group who exhibited a rather disorganized pattern. We discuss these results in terms of incoherence in the EEG continuity during sleep offset after prior sleep loss, which could partly account for the performance decrement observed during SI in sleep deprived subjects.


Subject(s)
Attention/physiology , Electroencephalography , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Sleep Deprivation/physiopathology , Sleep Stages/physiology , Wakefulness/physiology , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Arousal/physiology , Color , Evoked Potentials, Visual/physiology , Humans , Male , Polysomnography , Reaction Time/physiology , Sleep Deprivation/diagnosis
14.
Ind Health ; 42(1): 1-14, 2004 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14964612

ABSTRACT

Two types of methods based on a particular principle allow enhancing and sustaining workers' alertness all along their night work. The first one rather consists in arousing workers by exposing them to stimulant environment conditions (light or noise...) or by giving them natural or pharmacological reactivating substances (caffeine or amphetamines...) for example. The second principle consists in increasing workers' possibilities for resting and allowing them to have short sleep periods or Short Rest Periods (SRP) in an adapted area at the workplace. In order to use these techniques in real work situations, after a critical review taking into account both efficiency, advantages and disadvantages but also applicability and acceptability, the SRP technique stands out as the most efficient method as it has a certain number of advantages with regard to our initial objective.


Subject(s)
Arousal/physiology , Sleep , Work Schedule Tolerance , Central Nervous System Stimulants/administration & dosage , Humans , Hypnosis , Light , Music , Noise, Occupational , Temperature , Ventilation
15.
Percept Mot Skills ; 96(3 Pt 2): 1223-36, 2003 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12929776

ABSTRACT

Extensive research has demonstrated that shiftwork produces deleterious effects on health because of the desynchrony it induces in the biological clock. The problem is even more crucial for older workers who present, in addition, various decrements in their cognitive functioning, particularly on attention and memory. The present study assessed whether age was related to task complexity as a function of time of day and time-on-task in a rapid rotating work-rest schedule. 24 subjects (12 juniors: 20-30 years and 12 seniors: 50-60 years) performed either a simple task (visual discrimination) or a complex task (descending subtraction) on three different moments of the day simulating the main shifts (morning, evening, and night). Analysis indicated that an age effect was only present on the more complex task, which was demanding in attentional resources and memory load. Seniors had no deficit in performance on the simple task compared to juniors. The effect of time of day was restricted to the simple task for both age groups. However, some differential strategies appear to distinguish juniors and seniors, specifically on accuracy during the night, suggesting that subjects of different ages cope with cognitive tasks in different ways and that perhaps some adverse effects apparently associated with aging could be counteracted by efficient strategies, but not others.


Subject(s)
Chronobiology Phenomena/physiology , Circadian Rhythm , Cognition , Adult , Age Factors , Discrimination, Psychological , Female , Humans , Male , Mathematics , Memory , Middle Aged , Surveys and Questionnaires , Visual Perception
16.
Sleep Med Rev ; 4(4): 341-353, 2000 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12531174

ABSTRACT

Sleep inertia is a transitional state of lowered arousal occurring immediately after awakening from sleep and producing a temporary decrement in subsequent performance. Many factors are involved in the characteristics of sleep inertia. The duration of prior sleep can influence the severity of subsequent sleep inertia. Although most studies have focused on sleep inertia after short naps, its effects can be shown after a normal 8-h sleep period. One of the most critical factors is the sleep stage prior to awakening. Abrupt awakening during a slow wave sleep (SWS) episode produces more sleep inertia than awakening in stage 1 or 2, REM sleep being intermediate. Therefore, prior sleep deprivation usually enhances sleep inertia since it increases SWS. There is no direct evidence that sleep inertia exhibits a circadian rhythm. However, it seems that sleep inertia is more intense when awakening occurs near the trough of the core body temperature as compared to its circadian peak. A more controversial issue concerns the time course of sleep inertia. Depending on the studies, it can last from 1 min to 4 h. However, in the absence of major sleep deprivation, the duration of sleep inertia rarely exceeds 30 min. But all these results should be analysed as a function of type of task and dependent variables. Different cognitive functions are probably not sensitive to the same degree to sleep inertia and special attention should be provided to dependent variables as a result of the cognitive processes under review. Finally, sleep disorders represent risk factors which deserve new insight in treatment strategies to counteract the adverse effects of sleep inertia.

SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...