Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 20 de 21
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Am J Psychol ; 111(1): 33-42, 1998.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9624702

ABSTRACT

This study addressed two questions about metamemory accuracy: how feedback about recall performance affects confidence-recall accuracy and whether individual differences in confidence-recall accuracy are stable across different sets of test items. College students answered general knowledge questions and made confidence ratings about the correctness of the answers. Half the students were told whether their answers were correct. All students answered half the questions a second time. The results show that feedback did not produce a general improvement in metamemory accuracy; the improvement was specific to the questions for which feedback was provided. Also, individual students' metamemory accuracy showed moderate alternate-forms stability when each test was made up of 250 items. Therefore, researchers studying individual differences in metamemory accuracy on recall tasks should use tests that yield several hundred responses.


Subject(s)
Feedback , Memory/physiology , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Female , Humans , Male
2.
Mem Cognit ; 24(2): 226-34, 1996 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8881325

ABSTRACT

There are large individual differences in the degree of association between the accuracy of memories and subjective confidence in those memories. Are these differences stable within the same test, and between alternate forms of a test? In Experiment 1, college students were tested on 3 recognition memory tasks, then retested 2 weeks later on alternate forms of the same tasks. The relationship between confidence judgments and recognition performance displayed low split-half stability and low alternate-forms stability. A second experiment with elderly adults replicated these findings. In a third experiment, college students recalled answers to general knowledge questions and rated confidence in the correctness of each answer. Individual differences in the association between confidence and recall performance were not stable across the odd- and even-numbered items on the test. These data indicate the need for the development of procedures that will produce stable estimates of individuals' metacognitive accuracy.


Subject(s)
Individuality , Judgment , Mental Recall , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Verbal Learning , Adult , Attention , Discrimination Learning , Face , Female , Humans , Male , Retention, Psychology
3.
J Med Chem ; 38(1): 180-8, 1995 Jan 06.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7837229

ABSTRACT

A detailed structure-activity analysis was carried out using eight 1-alkyl(aralkyl)nipecotamides (type 5), 33 bis-nipecotamidoalkanes and aralkanes (type 6), and 7 N,N'-bis(nipecotoyl)-piperazines (type 7) as inhibitors of human platelet aggregation. Steric factors played an important role in determining the activity of type 5 compounds possessing an an appropriate degree of hydrophobic character. Types 6 and 7 compounds were more potent than the corresponding type 5 molecules. Hydrophobic character appeared to influence the activity of type 6 compounds. A 3-substituent on the piperidine ring was necessary for antiplatelet activity; the substituent should be preferably an amide with its C attached directly to the ring. 3,5-Disubstitution and 2-substitution led to a decline in activity. Optimal activity was attained when the two nipecotoyl ring N atoms were connected by an aralkyl group, and separated by approximately 7 A. It is suggested that van der Waals forces and pi interactions may govern the inhibitor-platelet interaction. The most potent type 6 inhibitor was alpha,alpha'-bis[3-(N-ethyl-N-butylcarbamoyl)piperidino]-p-xylene (6i). The most potent type 5 compound was 1-decyl-3-(N,N-diethylcarbamoyl)piperidine (5a). Any substitution on the piperazine ring of type 7 compounds led to a decline in activity, the most active analog being N,N'-bis(1-decylnipecotoyl)piperazine (7a). It is suggested that nipecotamides interact with anionic platelet sites located 7 A from each other and connected by a hydrophobic well.


Subject(s)
Amides/chemical synthesis , Amides/pharmacology , Nipecotic Acids/chemical synthesis , Nipecotic Acids/pharmacology , Piperidines/chemical synthesis , Piperidines/pharmacology , Platelet Aggregation Inhibitors/chemical synthesis , Platelet Aggregation Inhibitors/pharmacology , Adult , Chemical Phenomena , Chemistry, Physical , Female , Humans , Male , Structure-Activity Relationship
4.
Biol Cybern ; 66(4): 327-33, 1992.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1550882

ABSTRACT

Existing computational models of structure-from-motion--the appearance of three-dimensional motion generated by moving two-dimensional patterns--are all based on variations of optical flow or feature point correspondence within the interior of single objects. Three separate phenomena provide strong evidence that in human vision, structure-from-motion is significantly affected by surface boundary cues. In the first, a rotating cylinder is seen, though no variation in optical flow exists across the apparent cylinder. In the second, the shape of the bounding contour of a moving pattern dominates the actual differential motion within the pattern. In the third, the appearance of independently moving objects changes significantly when the boundary between them becomes indistinct. We describe a simple computational model sufficient to account for these effects. The model is based on qualitative constraints relating possible object motions to patterns of flow, together with an understanding of the patterns of flow that can be discriminated in practice.


Subject(s)
Visual Perception/physiology , Animals , Humans , Mathematics , Models, Biological , Rotation
5.
Biol Cybern ; 62(2): 113-6, 1989.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2597716

ABSTRACT

Active visual tracking of points on occlusion boundaries can simplify certain computations involved in determining scene structure and dynamics based on visual motion. Tracking is particularly effective at surface boundaries where large, discontinuous changes in depth are occurring. Two such techniques are described here. The first provides a measure of ordinal depth by distinguishing between occluding and occluded surfaces at a surface boundary. The second can be used to determine the direction of observer motion through a scene.


Subject(s)
Algorithms , Computer Simulation , Motion
6.
Laryngoscope ; 97(3 Pt 1): 267-70, 1987 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-3821344

ABSTRACT

Complication rates are lower using postoperative radiation therapy and cure rates at least compatible to preoperative radiation therapy. However, one of the concerns with postoperative radiation treatment is the possibility of delay in beginning the treatment because of an unhealed surgical wound. A delay of more than 6 weeks is detrimental. In order to study the effect of radiation therapy on incompletely healed wounds, a retrospective chart review of such cases during the period 1977 to 1984 was undertaken. One hundred and eighty-five patients had planned postoperative radiation therapy, and 13 of these began radiation therapy with an unhealed wound or fistula. Six of 10 wounds closed spontaneously, 4 required surgical closure, and 3 failed to heal. Seven patients in this study died with cancer, 2 died of other causes, 3 are alive without disease, and 1 patient remains alive with disease. We conclude that giving radiation therapy to an open wound with appropriate precaution can be done without serious complications.


Subject(s)
Head and Neck Neoplasms/surgery , Wound Healing/radiation effects , Adult , Aged , Combined Modality Therapy , Head and Neck Neoplasms/radiotherapy , Humans , Middle Aged , Time Factors
8.
IEEE Trans Pattern Anal Mach Intell ; 9(2): 229-44, 1987 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21869393

ABSTRACT

Multiple views of a scene can provide important information about the structure and dynamic behavior of three-dimensional objects. Many of the methods that recover this information require the determination of optical flow-the velocity, on the image, of visible points on object surfaces. An important class of techniques for estimating optical flow depend on the relationship between the gradients of image brightness. While gradient-based methods have been widely studied, little attention has been paid to accuracy and reliability of the approach. Gradient-based methods are sensitive to conditions commonly encountered in real imagery. Highly textured surfaces, large areas of constant brightness, motion boundaries, and depth discontinuities can all be troublesome for gradient-based methods. Fortunately, these problematic areas are usually localized can be identified in the image. In this paper we examine the sources of errors for gradient-based techniques that locally solve for optical flow. These methods assume that optical flow is constant in a small neighborhood. The consequence of violating in this assumption is examined. The causes of measurement errors and the determinants of the conditioning of the solution system are also considered. By understanding how errors arise, we are able to define the inherent limitations of the technique, obtain estimates of the accuracy of computed values, enhance the performance of the technique, and demonstrate the informative value of some types of error.

9.
IEEE Trans Pattern Anal Mach Intell ; 7(2): 133-8, 1985 Feb.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21869252

ABSTRACT

In dynamic scenes, the presence of object boundaries is often signaled by the appearance or disappearance of occluded surfaces over time. Such regions of surface accretion or deletion can be found using matching techniques similar to those used to determine optical flow in an image sequence. Regions in one frame that are not adequately matched by any region in previous frames correspond to accretion. Regions that have no matches in subsequent frames correspond to deletion. In either case, an occlusion boundary is present. Furthermore, by associating accretion or deletion regions with a surface on one side of a boundary, it is possible to determine which side of the boundary is being occluded. This association can be based purely on visual motion-the accretion or deletion region moves with the same image velocity as the remaining visible surface to which it is attached.

10.
IEEE Trans Pattern Anal Mach Intell ; 7(4): 374-83, 1985 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21869276

ABSTRACT

Optical flow can be used to locate dynamic occlusion boundaries in an image sequence. We derive an edge detection algorithm sensitive to changes in flow fields likely to be associated with occlusion. The algorithm is patterned after the Marr-Hildreth zero-crossing detectors currently used to locate boundaries in scalar fields. Zero-crossing detectors are extended to identify changes in direction and/or magnitude in a vector-valued flow field. As a result, the detector works for flow boundaries generated due to the relative motion of two overlapping surfaces, as well as the simpler case of motion parallax due to a sensor moving through an otherwise stationary environment. We then show how the approach can be extended to identify which side of a dynamic occlusion boundary corresponds to the occluding surface. The fundamental principal involved is that at an occlusion boundary, the image of the surface boundary moves with the image of the occluding surface. Such information is important in interpreting dynamic scenes. Results are demonstrated on optical flow fields automatically computed from real image sequences.

11.
IEEE Trans Pattern Anal Mach Intell ; 7(6): 713-7, 1985 Jun.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21869310

ABSTRACT

An important application of machine vision systems is the recognition of known three-dimensional objects. A major difficulty arises when two or more objects project the same or similar two-dimensional image, often resulting in misclassification and degradation of system performance. The changes in images which result from the motion of objects provide a source of three-dimensional information which can greatly aid the classification process, but this three-dimensional analysis is computationally complex and subject to many sources of error. This work develops a methodology which utilizes the information derived from the apparent changes in object features over time to facilitate the recognition task, without the need to actually recover the three-dimensional structure of the objects under view. The basic approach is to generate a ``feature signature'' by combining the feature measurements of the individual regions in a long sequence of images. The static information in the individual frames is analyzed along with the temporal information from the entire sequence. These techniques are particularly applicable in situations where static image processing methods cannot discriminate between ambiguous objects. Two example implementations are presented to illustrate the application of the techniques of object recognition using motion information.

12.
Brain Cogn ; 3(3): 239-48, 1984 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-6536328

ABSTRACT

Subjects made arousing or nonarousing judgments about photographs of strangers, then had an unannounced recognition test over the photographs. Emotional orienting tasks led to better retention than nonemotional tasks. Assuming emotionality is predominantly a right-hemisphere activity, then this result is consistent with other research showing a right-hemisphere advantage in processing faces. This difference was most clear for subjects with a right-hemisphere cognitive style. Extraversion had no effect on face recognition, nor did a subject's cognitive style seem to be associated with extraversion or neuroticism scores.


Subject(s)
Dominance, Cerebral , Emotions , Extraversion, Psychological , Form Perception , Memory , Mental Recall , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Adult , Arousal , Facial Expression , Humans , Set, Psychology
13.
Arch Oral Biol ; 27(7): 535-45, 1982.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-6751303

ABSTRACT

The indirect fluorescent-antibody technique was used to locate histidine-rich basic protein, filaggrin. In the newborn, immunofluorescence was seen in the cornified layers and in keratohyalin granules throughout the mouth using antibody specific for epidermal filaggrin, a distribution similar to that in epidermis where it is thought that filaggrin functions as the keratin matrix protein. In the adult immunofluorescence was in keratohyalin granules of palate, buccal and tongue epithelium but in the stratum corneum was limited to the soft palate with weak, patchy areas in the densely keratinized epithelium of the hard palate and tongue. Immunofluorescence was delineated at the boundary between the soft and hard palates. A protein apparently identical to epidermal filaggrin was identified in extracts of newborn palate by its mobility on sodium dodecyl sulphate-polyacrylamide gels and subsequent reaction with the antibody used for immunofluorescent studies. This protein was not detected in extracts of adult oral epithelia. Both newborn and adult tissues contained high mol. wt cross-reactive protein, suggestive of the filaggrin-precursor protein extractable from keratohyalin granules. The distribution of filaggrin was consistent with its function as a keratin matrix protein in the newborn oral epithelium and some less densely keratinized regions of the adult. However, in the adult mouth, filaggrin is not detectable in the stratum corneum of the most densely keratinized regions. Thus, the protein must be lost or its antigenic sites altered with maturation of the animal, depending on the type and extent of keratinization.


Subject(s)
Intermediate Filament Proteins , Mouth/analysis , Proteins/analysis , Aging , Animals , Animals, Newborn , Electrophoresis, Polyacrylamide Gel , Epidermis/analysis , Epithelium/analysis , Filaggrin Proteins , Fluorescent Antibody Technique , Rats , Rats, Inbred Strains
15.
IEEE Trans Pattern Anal Mach Intell ; 2(4): 333-40, 1980 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21868909

ABSTRACT

An algorithm for matching images of real world scenes is presented. The matching is a specification of the geometrical disparity between the images and may be used to partially reconstruct the three-dimensional structure of the scene. Sets of candidate matching points are selected independently in each image. These points are the locations of small, distinct features which are likely to be detectable in both images. An initial network of possible matches between the two sets of candidates is constructed. Each possible match specifies a possible disparity of a candidate point in a selected reference image. An initial estimate of the probability of each possible disparity is made, based on the similarity of subimages surrounding the points. These estimates are iteratively improved by a relaxation labeling technique making use of the local continuity property of disparity that is a consequence of the continuity of real world surfaces. The algorithm is effective for binocular parallax, motion parallax, and object motion. It quickly converges to good estimates of disparity, which reflect the spatial organization of the scene.

16.
Science ; 202(4368): 574, 1978 Nov 10.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17754018
17.
Invest Radiol ; 11(4): 258-66, 1976.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-783075

ABSTRACT

The results of two complementary approaches for performing diagnostic screening for the presence of coal workers' pneumoconiosis (CWP) from the routine posterior-anterior chest radiograph are presented. The first is a digital approach utilizing the measurement of image texture, while the second uses hybrid optical-digital methods involving the optical Fourier transform. Both approaches yield classification results comparable to experienced radiologists.


Subject(s)
Diagnosis, Computer-Assisted , Pneumoconiosis/diagnostic imaging , Coal Mining , Computers , Fourier Analysis , Humans , Male , Pattern Recognition, Automated , Radiography
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...