Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 10 de 10
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 80(1): 53-67, 2001 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11195891

ABSTRACT

Why do people's impulse controls break down during emotional distress? Some theories propose that distress impairs one's motivation or one's ability to exert self-control, and some postulate self-destructive intentions arising from the moods. Contrary to those theories, Three experiments found that believing that one's bad mood was frozen (unchangeable) eliminated the tendency to eat fattening snacks (Experiment 1), seek immediate gratification (Experiment 2), and engage in frivolous procrastination (Experiment 3). The implication is that when people are upset, they indulge immediate impulses to make themselves feel better, which amounts to giving short-term affect regulation priority over other self-regulatory goals.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Psychological , Affect , Feeding Behavior/psychology , Impulsive Behavior/psychology , Self Efficacy , Stress, Psychological , Adult , Analysis of Variance , Behavior Therapy/methods , Female , Humans , Male , Motivation
2.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 81(6): 1058-69, 2001 Dec.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11761307

ABSTRACT

Social exclusion was manipulated by telling people that they would end up alone later in life or that other participants had rejected them. These manipulations caused participants to behave more aggressively. Excluded people issued a more negative job evaluation against someone who insulted them (Experiments 1 and 2). Excluded people also blasted a target with higher levels of aversive noise both when the target had insulted them (Experiment 4) and when the target was a neutral person and no interaction had occurred (Experiment 5). However, excluded people were not more aggressive toward someone who issued praise (Experiment 3). These responseswere specific to social exclusion (as opposed to other misfortunes) and were not mediated by emotion


Subject(s)
Aggression , Social Alienation , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Interpersonal Relations , Male , Random Allocation , Surveys and Questionnaires
3.
J Soc Psychol ; 139(4): 446-57, 1999 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10457761

ABSTRACT

This study examined the results of repeated exercises of self-control in relation to self-regulatory strength over time. A sample of 69 U.S. college students spent 2 weeks doing 1 of 3 self-control exercises: monitoring and improving posture, regulating mood, or monitoring and recording eating. Compared with a no-exercise control group, the participants who performed the self-control exercises showed significant improvement in self-regulatory capacity as measured by quitting faster on a hand-grip exercise task following a thought-suppression exercise.


Subject(s)
Feeding Behavior/psychology , Posture , Self Concept , Thinking , Adult , Female , Hand Strength/physiology , Humans , Longitudinal Studies , Male , Random Allocation
4.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 74(5): 1252-65, 1998 May.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9599441

ABSTRACT

Choice, active response, self-regulation, and other volition may all draw on a common inner resource. In Experiment 1, people who forced themselves to eat radishes instead of tempting chocolates subsequently quit faster on unsolvable puzzles than people who had not had to exert self-control over eating. In Experiment 2, making a meaningful personal choice to perform attitude-relevant behavior caused a similar decrement in persistence. In Experiment 3, suppressing emotion led to a subsequent drop in performance of solvable anagrams. In Experiment 4, an initial task requiring high self-regulation made people more passive (i.e., more prone to favor the passive-response option). These results suggest that the self's capacity for active volition is limited and that a range of seemingly different, unrelated acts share a common resource.


Subject(s)
Choice Behavior , Ego , Internal-External Control , Volition , Adult , Affect , Attitude , Dependency, Psychological , Emotions , Female , Humans , Male , Problem Solving , Psychological Tests
5.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 74(3): 774-89, 1998 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9523419

ABSTRACT

If self-regulation conforms to an energy or strength model, then self-control should be impaired by prior exertion. In Study 1, trying to regulate one's emotional response to an upsetting movie was followed by a decrease in physical stamina. In Study 2, suppressing forbidden thoughts led to a subsequent tendency to give up quickly on unsolvable anagrams. In Study 3, suppressing thoughts impaired subsequent efforts to control the expression of amusement and enjoyment. In Study 4, autobiographical accounts of successful versus failed emotional control linked prior regulatory demands and fatigue to self-regulatory failure. A strength model of self-regulation fits the data better than activation, priming, skill, or constant capacity models of self-regulation.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Psychological , Internal-External Control , Mental Fatigue/psychology , Self Concept , Adult , Female , Frustration , Hand Strength , Humans , Male , Motivation , Problem Solving , Stress, Psychological/complications , Students/psychology , Thinking
6.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 64(1): 141-56, 1993 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8421250

ABSTRACT

The tendency for people with high self-esteem to make inflated assessments and predictions about themselves carries the risk of making commitments that exceed capabilities, thus leading to failure. Ss chose their performance contingencies in a framework where larger rewards were linked to a greater risk of failure. In the absence of ego threat, Ss with high self-esteem showed superior self-regulation: They set appropriate goals and performed effectively. Ego threat, however, caused Ss with high self-esteem to set inappropriate, risky goals that were beyond their performance capabilities so they ended up with smaller rewards than Ss with low self-esteem. The results indicate the danger of letting egotistical illusions interfere with self-regulation processes.


Subject(s)
Adaptation, Psychological , Ego , Internal-External Control , Self Concept , Adolescent , Adult , Aptitude , Goals , Humans , Male , Personality Development
7.
J Pers Soc Psychol ; 63(3): 435-51, 1992 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1403623

ABSTRACT

Studies 1 and 2 showed that identical behaviors had greater impact on the self-concept when performed publicly rather than privately. That is, the self-concept is more likely to change by internalizing public behavior than by internalizing behavior that is identical but lacks the interpersonal context. The self-concept change extends even to behavioral changes and occurs even when participants are unaware of being observed. In addition, those who are high in self-monitoring are more likely to internalize their behavior than those who are low in self-monitoring. Study 3 provided evidence about what components of a public situation affect the internalization of behavior. Choice about making the self-portrayal, drawing on episodes from one's own past rather than relying on a yoked script, and expecting future interaction with the audience all increased the internalization of a public behavior.


Subject(s)
Internal-External Control , Interpersonal Relations , Reality Testing , Self Concept , Adaptation, Psychological , Adult , Feedback , Female , Humans , Male , Personality
8.
Cortex ; 27(1): 81-92, 1991 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2055045

ABSTRACT

Feeling-of-Knowing was compared in left temporal, right temporal, and control subjects. Seizure patients, like controls, demonstrate knowledge of information that is not accessible to conscious recall, but they are less accurate in FOK judgements. Furthermore, there are significant differences between seizure patients and controls in the relationship between Feeling-of-Knowing and object level memory scores: (1) Controls spend more time searching memory for inaccessible information that is correctly identified on a later recognition test than they do for information that is incorrectly recognized. (2) Latency of correct recognition decreases as strength of FOK increases among controls, suggesting that controls correctly anticipate the outcome of memory search. These patterns of performance, which are consistent with an efficient memory search termination device, were not observed among seizure patients. The findings suggest that inaccurate Feeling-of-Knowing may play a role in inefficient memory search mechanisms in temporal lobe epilepsy.


Subject(s)
Awareness/physiology , Epilepsy, Temporal Lobe/physiopathology , Mental Recall/physiology , Adult , Attention/physiology , Dominance, Cerebral/physiology , Epilepsy, Temporal Lobe/psychology , Female , Humans , Male , Neuropsychological Tests , Semantics , Temporal Lobe/physiopathology , Verbal Learning/physiology
10.
J Pers ; 53(3): 407-24, 1985 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-4067788

ABSTRACT

In order to outline a model of identity crisis, it is necessary to distinguish two types. In an identity deficit ("motivation crisis"), the individual experiences a lack of guiding commitments but struggles to establish personal goals and values. In an identity conflict ("legitimation crisis"), the person has several commitments which prescribe conflicting behavioral imperatives in some situations, such that at least one commitment may have to be betrayed. The literature on identity crisis is reviewed in connection with this distinction, and an attempt is made to delineate the causes, the subjective experiences, behavioral consequences, and modes of resolution of each type of crisis.


Subject(s)
Identity Crisis , Adaptation, Psychological , Adolescent , Adult , Conflict, Psychological , Ego , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Motivation , Parent-Child Relations , Personality Development , Social Values
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...