Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 3 de 3
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
J Am Board Fam Med ; 34(6): 1229-1242, 2021.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34772779

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Post-COVID symptoms, defined as symptoms lasting >4 weeks postinfection, have been identified not only among those patients who were hospitalized with severe symptoms but also among those who were asymptomatic or with only mild symptoms. Primary care providers (PCPs) will often be the first point of contact for patients experiencing potential complications of post-COVID symptoms. The aim of this article is to present a post-COVID management tool for PCPs to use as a quick reference and guide to the initial workup and management of the most common post-COVID symptoms. METHODS: Published guidance, recent literature, and expert specialist opinion were used to create the structure outlining the outpatient evaluation and treatment for post-COVID symptoms. RESULTS: A quick-reference guide for management of post-COVID symptoms was created for PCPs. Educational materials were created for clinicians to share with patients. Our article reviews several common complaints including respiratory, cognitive, and neurological symptoms, chronic fatigue, dysautonomia, and anosmia and presents recommendations for management. CONCLUSIONS: Data on long-term effects of COVID-19 are still emerging, and rapid dissemination of this data to front-line PCPs is crucial. This table was our effort to make the currently available evidence accessible for our PCPs in a simple, easy-to-use format.


Subject(s)
COVID-19 , Physicians, Primary Care , Humans , SARS-CoV-2
2.
Elife ; 102021 10 26.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34696826

ABSTRACT

Visual predators rely on fast-acting optokinetic responses to track and capture agile prey. Most toothed whales, however, rely on echolocation for hunting and have converged on biosonar clicking rates reaching 500/s during prey pursuits. If echoes are processed on a click-by-click basis, as assumed, neural responses 100× faster than those in vision are required to keep pace with this information flow. Using high-resolution biologging of wild predator-prey interactions, we show that toothed whales adjust clicking rates to track prey movement within 50-200 ms of prey escape responses. Hypothesising that these stereotyped biosonar adjustments are elicited by sudden prey accelerations, we measured echo-kinetic responses from trained harbour porpoises to a moving target and found similar latencies. High biosonar sampling rates are, therefore, not supported by extreme speeds of neural processing and muscular responses. Instead, the neurokinetic response times in echolocation are similar to those of tracking responses in vision, suggesting a common neural underpinning.


In the animal world, split-second decisions determine whether a predator eats, or its prey survives. There is a strong evolutionary advantage to fast reacting brains and bodies. For example, the eye muscles of hunting cheetahs must lock on to a gazelle and keep track of it, no matter how quickly or unpredictably it moves. In fact, in monkeys and primates, these muscles can react to sudden movements in as little as 50 milliseconds ­ faster than the blink of an eye. But what about animals that do not rely on vision to hunt? To find food at night or in the deep ocean, whales and porpoises make short ultrasonic sounds, or 'clicks', and then listen for returning echoes. As they close in on a prey, they need to click faster to get quicker updates on its location. What is unclear is how fast they react to the echoes. Just before a kill, a harbour porpoise can click over 500 times a second: if they wait for the echo from one click before making the next one, they would need responses 100 times faster than human eyes. Exploring this topic is difficult, as it requires tracking predator and prey at the same time. Vance et al. took up the challenge by building sound and movement recorders that attach to whales with suction cups. These were used on two different hunters: deep-diving beaked whales and shallow-hunting harbour porpoises. Both species adapted their click rate depending on how far they were from their prey, but their response times were similar to visual responses in monkeys and humans. This means that whales and porpoises do not act on each echo before clicking again: instead, they respond to groups of tens of clicks at a time. This suggests that their brains may be wired in much the same way as the ones of visual animals. In the ocean, increased human activity creates a dangerous noise pollution that disrupts the delicate hunting mechanism of whales and porpoises. Better understanding how these animals find their food may therefore help conservation efforts.


Subject(s)
Echolocation , Predatory Behavior , Whales/physiology , Acoustics , Animals , Biomechanical Phenomena , Kinetics
3.
J Am Board Fam Med ; 29(1): 24-8, 2016.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26769874

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Although safe and effective, intrauterine devices (IUDs) are underutilized in the United States. The objective of this study was to determine whether patient- and clinician-initiated discussions were associated with the duration of Mirena (52 mg levonorgestrel-releasing) IUD use. METHODS: Retrospective chart review identified Mirena IUDs that were placed and removed for reasons other than the desire for pregnancy from January 1, 2005, to January 1, 2012, at 1 academic center (n = 148). Multivariable linear regression examined the independent association between duration of use and source of discussion initiation. RESULTS: IUDs placed after patient-initiated discussions were used for 473 days longer than those placed after clinician-initiated discussions (P < .001). CONCLUSION: Patient-initiated discussions of Mirena IUDs are associated with a significantly longer duration of use. Patient influences outside the clinical encounter may be important in optimizing use.


Subject(s)
Intrauterine Devices, Medicated/statistics & numerical data , Patient Preference/statistics & numerical data , Practice Patterns, Physicians'/statistics & numerical data , Adult , Communication , Contraceptive Agents, Female/administration & dosage , Contraceptive Agents, Female/adverse effects , Counseling , Female , Humans , Intrauterine Devices, Medicated/adverse effects , Levonorgestrel/administration & dosage , Linear Models , Medical Records , Patient Preference/psychology , Physician-Patient Relations , Pregnancy , Retrospective Studies , Time Factors , United States
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...