Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 19 de 19
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 120(22): e2214930120, 2023 05 30.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37216543

ABSTRACT

It is widely believed that observers can fail to notice clearly visible unattended objects, even if they are moving. Here, we created parametric tasks to test this belief and report the results of three high-powered experiments (total n = 4,493) indicating that this effect is strongly modulated by the speed of the unattended object. Specifically, fast-but not slow-objects are readily noticeable, whether they are attended or not. These results suggest that fast motion serves as a potent exogenous cue that overrides task-focused attention, showing that fast speeds, not long exposure duration or physical salience, strongly diminish inattentional blindness effects.


Subject(s)
Gorilla gorilla , Visual Perception , Humans , Animals , Attention , Cognition , Blindness
2.
Sci Rep ; 12(1): 9133, 2022 06 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35650220

ABSTRACT

Previous research has uncovered medical and psychological effects of anabolic-androgenic steroid (AAS) use, but the specific relationship between AAS use and risk-taking behaviors as well as between AAS use and psychopathic tendencies remains understudied. To explore these potential relationships, we anonymously recruited 492 biologically male, self-identified bodybuilders (median age 22; range 18-47 years) from online bodybuilding fora to complete an online survey on Appearance and Performance Enhancing Drug (APED) use, psychological traits, lifestyle choices, and health behaviors. We computed odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals using logistic regression, adjusting for age, race, education, exercise frequency, caloric intake, and lean BMI. Bodybuilders with a prior history of AAS use exhibited heightened odds of psychopathic traits, sexual and substance use risk-taking behaviors, anger problems, and physical problems compared to those with no prior history of AAS use. This study is among the first to directly assess psychopathy within AAS users. Our results on risk-taking, anger problems, and physical problems are consistent with prior AAS research as well as with existing frameworks of AAS use as a risk behavior. Future research should focus on ascertaining causality, specifically whether psychopathy is a risk associated with or a result of AAS use.


Subject(s)
Anabolic Agents , Adolescent , Adult , Anabolic Agents/adverse effects , Anger , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Risk-Taking , Steroids , Testosterone Congeners/adverse effects , Young Adult
3.
STAR Protoc ; 3(2): 101248, 2022 06 17.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35310080

ABSTRACT

The ability to synchronize a motor action to a rhythmic auditory stimulus is often considered an innate human skill. However, some individuals lack the ability to synchronize speech to a perceived syllabic rate. Here, we describe a simple and fast protocol to classify a single native English speaker as being or not being a speech synchronizer. This protocol consists of four parts: the pretest instructions and volume adjustment, the training procedure, the execution of the main task, and data analysis. For complete details on the use and execution of this protocol, please refer to Assaneo et al. (2019a).


Subject(s)
Acoustic Stimulation , Speech , Humans
4.
Iperception ; 12(3): 20416695211018720, 2021.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34777780

ABSTRACT

Here, we introduce and explore Scintillating Starbursts, a stimulus type made up of concentric star polygons that induce illusory scintillating rays or beams. We test experimentally which factors, such as contrast and number of vertices, modulate how observers experience this stimulus class. We explain how the illusion arises from the interplay of known visual processes, specifically central versus peripheral vision, and interpret the phenomenology evoked by these patterns. We discuss how Starbursts differ from similar and related visual illusions such as illusory contours, grid illusions such as the pincushion grid illusion as well as moiré patterns.

5.
J Undergrad Neurosci Educ ; 19(2): A185-A191, 2021.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34552436

ABSTRACT

The 2019 Society for Neuroscience Professional Development Workshop on Teaching reviewed current tools, approaches, and examples for teaching computation in neuroscience. Robert Kass described the statistical foundations that students need to properly analyze data. Pascal Wallisch compared MATLAB and Python as programming languages for teaching students. Adrienne Fairhall discussed computational methods, training opportunities, and curricular considerations. Walt Babiec provided a view from the trenches on practical aspects of teaching computational neuroscience. Mathew Abrams concluded the session with an overview of resources for teaching and learning computational modeling in neuroscience.

6.
J Neurophysiol ; 122(5): 1937-1945, 2019 11 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31509468

ABSTRACT

Response properties of MT neurons are often studied with "bikinetic" plaid stimuli, which consist of two superimposed sine wave gratings moving in different directions. Oculomotor studies using "unikinetic plaids" in which only one of the two superimposed gratings moves suggest that the eyes first move reflexively in the direction of the moving grating and only later converge on the perceived direction of the moving pattern. MT has been implicated as the source of visual signals that drives these responses. We wanted to know whether stationary gratings, which have little effect on MT cells when presented alone, would influence MT responses when paired with a moving grating. We recorded extracellularly from neurons in area MT and measured responses to stationary and moving gratings, and to their sums: bikinetic and unikinetic plaids. As expected, stationary gratings presented alone had a very modest influence on the activity of MT neurons. Responses to moving gratings and bikinetic plaids were similar to those previously reported and revealed cells selective for the motion of plaid patterns and of their components (pattern and component cells). When these neurons were probed with unikinetic plaids, pattern cells shifted their direction preferences in a way that revealed the influence of the static grating. Component cell preferences shifted little or not at all. These results support the notion that pattern-selective neurons in area MT integrate component motions that differ widely in speed, and that they do so in a way that is consistent with an intersection-of-constraints model.NEW & NOTEWORTHY Human perceptual and eye movement responses to moving gratings are influenced by adding a second, static grating to create a "unikinetic" plaid. Cells in MT do not respond to static gratings, but those gratings still influence the direction selectivity of some MT cells. The cells influenced by static gratings are those tuned for the motion of global patterns, but not those tuned only for the individual components of moving targets.


Subject(s)
Electrophysiological Phenomena/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Neurons/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Visual/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Visual Cortex/physiology , Animals , Behavior, Animal/physiology , Female , Macaca fascicularis , Macaca mulatta , Male
7.
PLoS One ; 14(2): e0210066, 2019.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30726220

ABSTRACT

How well do we remember popular music? To investigate how hit songs are recognized over time, we randomly selected number-one Billboard singles from the last 76 years and presented them to a large sample of mostly millennial participants. In response to hearing each song, participants were prompted to indicate whether they recognized it. Plotting the recognition proportion for each song as a function of the year during which it reached peak popularity resulted in three distinct phases in collective memory. The first phase is characterized by a steep linear drop-off in recognition for the music from this millennium; the second phase consists of a stable plateau during the 1960s to the 1990s; and the third phase, a further but more gradual drop-off during the 1940s and 1950s. More than half of recognition variability can be accounted for by self-selected exposure to each song as measured by its play count on Spotify. We conclude that collective memory for popular music is different from that of other historical phenomena.


Subject(s)
Auditory Perception/physiology , Mental Recall/physiology , Music , Adolescent , Adult , Female , Humans , Male
8.
J Vis ; 17(4): 5, 2017 04 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28388701

ABSTRACT

There has been considerable interest in a stimulus ("the dress") that yields starkly divergent subjective color percepts between observers. It has been proposed that individual differences in the subjective interpretation of this stimulus are due to the different assumptions that individuals make about how the dress was illuminated. In this study, we address this possible explanation empirically by reporting on data from ∼13,000 observers who were surveyed online. We show that assumptions about the illumination of the dress-i.e., whether the stimulus was illuminated by natural or artificial light or whether it was in a shadow-strongly affects the subjective interpretation of observers, compared to demographic factors, such as age or gender, which have a relatively smaller influence. We interpret these findings in a Bayesian framework by also showing that prior exposure to long- or short-wavelength lights due to circadian type shapes the subjective experience of the dress stimulus in theoretically expected ways.


Subject(s)
Aging/physiology , Color Perception/physiology , Individuality , Lighting , Adult , Aged , Bayes Theorem , Female , Humans , Male , Middle Aged , Psychomotor Performance/physiology
9.
J Neurosci ; 36(44): 11238-11258, 2016 11 02.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27807166

ABSTRACT

Synaptic plasticity is sensitive to the rate and the timing of presynaptic and postsynaptic action potentials. In experimental protocols inducing plasticity, the imposed spike trains are typically regular and the relative timing between every presynaptic and postsynaptic spike is fixed. This is at odds with firing patterns observed in the cortex of intact animals, where cells fire irregularly and the timing between presynaptic and postsynaptic spikes varies. To investigate synaptic changes elicited by in vivo-like firing, we used numerical simulations and mathematical analysis of synaptic plasticity models. We found that the influence of spike timing on plasticity is weaker than expected from regular stimulation protocols. Moreover, when neurons fire irregularly, synaptic changes induced by precise spike timing can be equivalently induced by a modest firing rate variation. Our findings bridge the gap between existing results on synaptic plasticity and plasticity occurring in vivo, and challenge the dominant role of spike timing in plasticity. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT: Synaptic plasticity, the change in efficacy of connections between neurons, is thought to underlie learning and memory. The dominant paradigm posits that the precise timing of neural action potentials (APs) is central for plasticity induction. This concept is based on experiments using highly regular and stereotyped patterns of APs, in stark contrast with natural neuronal activity. Using synaptic plasticity models, we investigated how irregular, in vivo-like activity shapes synaptic plasticity. We found that synaptic changes induced by precise timing of APs are much weaker than suggested by regular stimulation protocols, and can be equivalently induced by modest variations of the AP rate alone. Our results call into question the dominant role of precise AP timing for plasticity in natural conditions.


Subject(s)
Action Potentials/physiology , Models, Neurological , Neuronal Plasticity/physiology , Neurons/physiology , Synapses/physiology , Synaptic Transmission/physiology , Animals , Computer Simulation , Humans , Models, Statistical , Nerve Net/physiology
10.
J Neurophysiol ; 115(1): 434-44, 2016 Jan 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26445864

ABSTRACT

The repeated presentation of an identical visual stimulus in the receptive field of a neuron may evoke different spiking patterns at each trial. Probabilistic methods are essential to understand the functional role of this variance within the neural activity. In that case, a Poisson process is the most common model of trial-to-trial variability. For a Poisson process, the variance of the spike count is constrained to be equal to the mean, irrespective of the duration of measurements. Numerous studies have shown that this relationship does not generally hold. Specifically, a majority of electrophysiological recordings show an "overdispersion" effect: responses that exhibit more intertrial variability than expected from a Poisson process alone. A model that is particularly well suited to quantify overdispersion is the Negative-Binomial distribution model. This model is well-studied and widely used but has only recently been applied to neuroscience. In this article, we address three main issues. First, we describe how the Negative-Binomial distribution provides a model apt to account for overdispersed spike counts. Second, we quantify the significance of this model for any neurophysiological data by proposing a statistical test, which quantifies the odds that overdispersion could be due to the limited number of repetitions (trials). We apply this test to three neurophysiological data sets along the visual pathway. Finally, we compare the performance of this model to the Poisson model on a population decoding task. We show that the decoding accuracy is improved when accounting for overdispersion, especially under the hypothesis of tuned overdispersion.


Subject(s)
Binomial Distribution , Geniculate Bodies/physiology , Models, Neurological , Neurons/physiology , Visual Cortex/physiology , Action Potentials , Animals , Female , Macaca mulatta , Male , Mice , Photic Stimulation , Poisson Distribution
11.
J Neurophysiol ; 111(11): 2332-42, 2014 Jun 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24647430

ABSTRACT

Self-motion generates patterns of optic flow on the retina. Neurons in the dorsal part of the medial superior temporal area (MSTd) are selective for these optic flow patterns. It has been shown that neurons in this area that are selective for expanding optic flow fields are involved in heading judgments. We wondered how subpopulations of MSTd neurons, those tuned for expansion, rotation or spiral motion, contribute to heading perception. To investigate this question, we recorded from neurons in area MSTd with diverse tuning properties, while the animals performed a heading-discrimination task. We found a significant trial-to-trial correlation (choice probability) between the MSTd neurons and the animals' decision. Neurons in different subpopulations did not differ significantly in terms of their choice probability. Instead, choice probability was strongly related to the sensitivity of the neuron in our sample, regardless of tuning preference. We conclude that a variety of subpopulations of MSTd neurons with different tuning properties contribute to heading judgments.


Subject(s)
Choice Behavior/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Optic Flow/physiology , Orientation/physiology , Sensory Receptor Cells/physiology , Temporal Lobe/physiology , Visual Fields/physiology , Animals , Judgment/physiology , Macaca mulatta , Male , Nerve Net/physiology , Task Performance and Analysis
13.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 75(5): 1039-47, 2013 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23456973

ABSTRACT

Motion aftereffects (MAEs) are thought to result from the adaptation of both subcortical and cortical systems involved in the processing of visual motion. Recently, it has been reported that the implied motion of static images in combination with linguistic descriptions of motion is sufficient to elicit an MAE, although neither factor alone is thought to directly activate visual motion areas in the brain. Given that the monotonic change of musical pitch is widely recognized in music as a metaphor for vertical motion, we investigated whether prolonged exposure to ascending or descending musical scales can also produce a visual motion aftereffect. After listening to ascending or descending musical scales, participants made decisions about the direction of visual motion in random-dot kinematogram stimuli. Metaphoric motion in the musical stimuli did affect the visual direction judgments, in that repeated exposure to rising or falling musical scales shifted participants' sensitivity to visual motion in the opposite direction. The finding that music can induce an MAE suggests that the subjective interpretation of monotonic pitch change as motion may have a perceptual foundation.


Subject(s)
Illusions/physiology , Judgment/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Music , Pitch Perception/physiology , Adaptation, Physiological/physiology , Adult , Female , Humans , Male , Metaphor , Models, Psychological , Motion , Psychoacoustics , Space Perception/physiology , Young Adult
14.
J Neurosci ; 32(24): 8242-53, 2012 Jun 13.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22699905

ABSTRACT

Neurons in area MT are sensitive to the direction of motion of gratings and of plaids made by summing 2 gratings moving in different directions. MT component direction-selective (CDS) neurons respond to the individual gratings of a plaid. Pattern direction-selective (PDS) neurons on the other hand, combine component information and respond selectively to the resulting pattern motion. Adding a third grating creates a "triplaid," which contains 3 grating and 3 plaid motions and is perceptually multistable. To examine how direction-selective mechanisms parse the motion signals in triplaids, we recorded MT responses of anesthetized and awake macaques to stimuli in which 3 identical moving gratings whose directions were separated by 120° were introduced in 3 successive epochs, going from grating to plaid to triplaid. CDS and PDS neurons-selected based on their responses to gratings and plaids-had strikingly different tuning properties in the triplaid epoch. CDS neurons were strongly tuned for the direction of motion of individual gratings, but PDS neurons nearly lost their selectivity for either the gratings or the plaids in the stimulus. We explain this reduced motion selectivity with a model that relates pattern selectivity of PDS neurons to a broad pooling of V1 afferents with a near-cosine weighting profile. Because PDS neurons signal both component and pattern motion in gratings and plaids, their reduced selectivity for motion in triplaids may be what makes these stimuli perceptually multistable.


Subject(s)
Motion Perception/physiology , Visual Pathways/physiology , Action Potentials/physiology , Animals , Female , Fixation, Ocular/physiology , Macaca , Male , Neurons/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Visual Cortex/physiology , Wakefulness/physiology
16.
Neuron ; 60(2): 195-7, 2008 Oct 23.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18957212

ABSTRACT

In this issue of Neuron, Chowdhury and DeAngelis report that training monkeys to perform a fine depth discrimination abolishes the contribution of signals from area MT to the execution of a different, coarse depth discrimination. This result calls into question the principle of associating particular visual areas with particular visual functions, by showing that such associations are modifiable by experience.


Subject(s)
Depth Perception/physiology , Discrimination Learning/physiology , Macaca/physiology , Temporal Lobe/physiology , Visual Cortex/physiology , Action Potentials/physiology , Animals , Brain Mapping , Nerve Net/physiology , Neurons/physiology , Temporal Lobe/anatomy & histology , Visual Cortex/anatomy & histology , Visual Pathways/anatomy & histology , Visual Pathways/physiology
17.
Vis Neurosci ; 25(2): 187-95, 2008.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18442441

ABSTRACT

Motion transparency occurs when multiple object velocities are present within a local region of retinotopic space. Transparent signals can carry information useful in the segmentation of moving objects and in the extraction of three-dimensional structure from relative motion cues. However, the physiological substrate underlying the detection of motion transparency is poorly understood. Direction tuned neurons in area MT are suppressed by transparent stimuli, suggesting that other motion sensitive areas may be needed to represent this signal robustly. Recent neuroimaging evidence implicated two such areas in the macaque superior temporal sulcus. We studied one of these, FST, with electrophysiological methods and found that a large fraction of the neurons responded well to two opposite directions of motion and to transparent stimuli containing those same directions. A linear combination of MT-like responses qualitatively reproduces this behavior and predicts that FST neurons can be tuned for transparent motion containing specific direction and depth components. We suggest that FST plays a role in motion segmentation based on transparent signals.


Subject(s)
Macaca mulatta/physiology , Motion Perception/physiology , Orientation/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Temporal Lobe/physiology , Animals , Electrophysiology , Female , Male , Models, Neurological , Neurons/physiology , Photic Stimulation/methods , Temporal Lobe/cytology
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...