Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 10 de 10
Filter
Add more filters










Publication year range
1.
Am J Addict ; 2018 Jul 19.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30024076

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVES: Delay discounting is associated with numerous clinically significant aspects of substance use disorders (SUDs). Recent studies have demonstrated that different models for assessing discounting may result in disparate conclusions. The current study compared two discounting tasks: money now versus money later (M-M) and methamphetamine now versus money later (MA-M) among non-treatment seeking individuals (N = 59) with methamphetamine use disorder (MAUD). Results from each task were assessed using three different models for assessing delay discounting. METHODS: Discounting data were fit to three models of discounting, log k using Mazur's hyperbolic formula, area under the curve (AUC), and an alternative AUC model in which the delay values have been log transformed (AUClog). RESULTS: For both discounting tasks, the distribution of model-related outcomes were normally distributed when using log k and AUClog, but skewed for AUC. Discounting in the MA-M task was significantly greater compared to the M-M task when using log k and AUClog but not AUC. CONCLUSION: To our knowledge, the current study is the first to report significantly greater discounting in a MA-M relative to M-M discounting task among individuals with MAUD, an outcome consistent with other psychomotor stimulants and drugs of abuse. SCIENTIFIC SIGNIFICANCE: The differential results observed across the three discounting models reaffirm potential issues with AUC noted in recent studies and highlight that researchers must be cautious when deciding on their final model of discounting. (Am J Addict 2018;XX:1-8).

2.
Psychol Rec ; 67(3): 355-363, 2017 09.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28970640

ABSTRACT

Delay discounting describes how a reward loses value as a function of increasing delay to its receipt and has been reliably associated with a variety of vulnerable populations including those with substance use disorders (SUDs). Two commonly used models to assess delay discounting in the field of SUDs include log k derived from Mazur's hyperbolic equation and area under the curve (AUC). In the current study, we compared log k with AUC on delay discounting data obtained from non-treatment seeking, cocaine- and methamphetamine-dependent volunteers. We specifically chose this population in order to obtain a distribution of relatively steep discounters. The results show that the relationship between AUC and log k is better described by a quadratic rather than a linear function. In other words, changes in discounting, as measured by AUC and log k, are reflected differently across a range of obtained responses. Additionally, the distribution of AUC values was skewed, which appears to be more likely among populations exhibiting greater discounting. Finally, closer examination of indifference points revealed that AUC was almost perfectly predicted by the area from the two longest delays, with relatively less input from shorter delays. Given these results, researchers should exercise additional caution when deciding which method to assess discounting data and how final results are to be interpreted, particularly when dealing with relatively high rates of discounting. High rates of discounting are likely in populations with impulsive disorders such as those with SUDs.

3.
Psychopharmacology (Berl) ; 232(5): 975-83, 2015 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25209677

ABSTRACT

RATIONALE: Varenicline (VAR), a smoking cessation aid that is a partial agonist at nicotinic receptors, mimics the reinforcement-enhancing effects of nicotine. Varenicline, when accompanied by non-drug cues, is self-administered by rats, though it is unclear whether this results from varenicline acting as a primary reinforcer or a reinforcement enhancer of the cues. OBJECTIVES: This study sought to disentangle these two potential actions. METHODS: Rats were allowed to self-administer intravenous nicotine, saline, or varenicline during 1-h sessions in operant chambers equipped with two levers. Five groups had concurrent access to drug infusions and a moderately reinforcing visual stimulus (VS) for responding on separate levers. Meeting the reinforcement schedule on one lever was reinforced with VAR (0.01, 0.06, 0.1 mg/kg/infusion), nicotine (0.06 mg/kg/infusion), or saline, while meeting the same schedule on the other lever delivered the VS. Additional groups were reinforced for pressing a single "active" lever and received VAR paired with the VS, the VS with response-independent infusions of VAR, or VAR alone (0.1 mg/kg/infusion). RESULTS: Rats readily responded for VAR paired with VS on a single lever. However, when VAR was the only reinforcer contingent on a response, rats did not respond more than for saline. CONCLUSIONS: These findings show that VAR does not serve as a primary reinforcer in rats at doses that increase responding for non-drug reinforcers. These data are consistent with research showing that the primary reinforcing effects of VAR are weak, at best, and that the primary reinforcing and reinforcement-enhancing actions of nicotinic drugs are pharmacologically distinct.


Subject(s)
Benzazepines/administration & dosage , Conditioning, Operant/drug effects , Nicotine/administration & dosage , Nicotinic Agonists/administration & dosage , Quinoxalines/administration & dosage , Animals , Cues , Dose-Response Relationship, Drug , Male , Rats , Rats, Sprague-Dawley , Reinforcement Schedule , Reinforcement, Psychology , Self Administration , Smoking Cessation , Varenicline
4.
Pharmacol Biochem Behav ; 124: 320-5, 2014 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24967870

ABSTRACT

Nicotine has been shown to enhance the motivational properties of non-nicotine stimuli. This reinforcement-enhancing property of nicotine has the potential to promote the use of other illicit substances as well as maladaptive patterns of food intake. Therefore, the current study aimed to examine whether nicotine enhances preference for contexts paired with cocaine or sucrose utilizing a place conditioning procedure. Separate groups of adult male rats were administered sucrose or cocaine in one of two compartments of a standard CPP chamber on four consecutive days. Preference was then assessed following no injection, a single subcutaneous (s.c.) injection of nicotine, and a s.c. saline injection. The animals preferred the chamber paired with either sucrose or cocaine, as evident from an increased time spent in the paired chamber compared to baseline. Nicotine further increased the time spent in the sucrose- or cocaine-paired chamber, consistent with a reinforcement-enhancement effect. Previous results demonstrate an interaction between nicotine and intake of other drugs or food. The present findings provide an additional mechanism that may underlie these effects and which may have implications for drug dependence and obesity.


Subject(s)
Cocaine/administration & dosage , Conditioning, Classical , Nicotine/pharmacology , Sucrose/administration & dosage , Animals , Male , Rats , Rats, Sprague-Dawley
5.
Drug Alcohol Depend ; 125(3): 307-12, 2012 Oct 01.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22483812

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Adolescence is a period of development associated with a peak in an organism's responsiveness to reward. Epidemiological data indicate that the initiation of smoking is high during adolescence and that earlier age of onset is associated with increased incidence of dependence as adults. In rats, nicotine is known to have primary reinforcing and reinforcement enhancing effects. Although the primary reinforcing effects of nicotine have been demonstrated in adolescent rats (self-administration), less is known about its reinforcement enhancing effects during this period. Moreover, the impact of adolescent nicotine exposure on its reinforcement enhancing effects during adulthood has not yet been examined. The objectives of this study were to assess whether (1) nicotine enhances operant responding for an unconditioned visual reinforcer (VS) in adolescent rats, and (2) exposure to nicotine during adolescence affects responsiveness to the VS in adulthood. METHODS: Rats were exposed to nicotine (0.32 mg/kg, subcutaneous injection) or saline during adolescence (postnatal day 29-42) and adulthood. Nose-poking for the VS was assessed under fixed and progressive ratio schedules. RESULTS: Nicotine increased responding for the VS during adolescence. Adolescent nicotine exposure failed to significantly affect adult responsiveness for the VS, regardless of adult nicotine exposure, but early exposure to the VS affected responsiveness to the VS in adulthood. CONCLUSIONS: Nicotine exhibits reinforcement enhancing effects in adolescent rats. Long-term effects of adolescent nicotine on reinforcement enhancement are minimal, but the impact of early exposure to the VS and/or the primary reinforcing effects of nicotine requires further investigation.


Subject(s)
Conditioning, Operant/drug effects , Nicotine/pharmacology , Nicotinic Agonists/pharmacology , Reinforcement, Psychology , Aging/psychology , Analysis of Variance , Animals , Cues , Male , Photic Stimulation , Rats , Rats, Sprague-Dawley , Reinforcement Schedule
6.
Nicotine Tob Res ; 14(7): 824-32, 2012 Jul.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22218403

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: Research has identified at least two positive reinforcement-related effects of nicotine: (a) primary reinforcement and (b) enhancement of reinforcement from concurrently available stimuli. Prior examples of the reinforcement-enhancing effects with rats showed that repeated, intermittent nicotine exposure increased responding for non-nicotine reinforcers, and this effect remained robust over several weeks. However, the effects of continuous nicotine exposure on responding for a non-nicotine reinforcer are unknown, as are the effects of abruptly withdrawing continuous nicotine on behavior maintained by the same reinforcer. METHODS: Lever pressing for a visual reinforcer under a fixed ratio schedule was assessed while rats were maintained on a chronic, continuous infusion of nicotine (3.16 mg/kg/day; osmotic minipump). The effects of precipitated withdrawal on responding, following 16 days of continuous nicotine exposure, were assessed by pre-session subcutaneous injections of mecamylamine (1.0 mg/kg). RESULTS: Continuous nicotine initially increased active responding for the visual reinforcer; however, continued exposure resulted in an attenuation of this effect. Precipitated withdrawal from nicotine resulted in a significant decline in active responding. CONCLUSIONS: The initial increase in responding for the visual reinforcer with chronic nicotine exposure is consistent with prior research showing that intermittent exposure to nicotine acts as a reinforcement enhancer. However, the attenuation of this enhancement following prolonged nicotine exposure is in contrast with the persistent effects previously reported. Finally, the decrease in visual reinforcers below control levels (nicotine-naive animals) following nicotine withdrawal highlights a potential for affective withdrawal, which may serve as a motive for continued nicotine use.


Subject(s)
Nicotine/pharmacology , Photic Stimulation , Reinforcement, Psychology , Substance Withdrawal Syndrome/psychology , Animals , Conditioning, Operant , Injections, Subcutaneous , Male , Mecamylamine/pharmacology , Nicotine/administration & dosage , Nicotine/antagonists & inhibitors , Nicotinic Antagonists/pharmacology , Rats , Rats, Sprague-Dawley , Reinforcement Schedule , Time Factors
7.
Nicotine Tob Res ; 14(3): 299-305, 2012 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21994342

ABSTRACT

INTRODUCTION: Varenicline (VAR), a partial nicotinic agonist, is one of the most effective smoking cessation pharmacotherapies. The therapeutic efficacy of VAR could be partly the result of substituting for and/or blocking the reinforcement-enhancing effects of nicotine (NIC). We assessed the effects of VAR alone and in combination with NIC (0.4 mg/kg) while rats pressed the lever for a moderately reinforcing visual stimulus (VS). METHODS: Rats were injected with placebo (0.9% saline), NIC, VAR (0.1-1 mg/kg), or NIC + VAR. A follow-up study was conducted with a broader dose range of VAR-alone dosages (0.01-3.0 mg/kg). All drug manipulations were conducted in a between-subjects design to prevent confounding effects of repeated exposure. RESULTS: There was a dose-dependent effect of VAR alone. Moderate doses of VAR (0.1 and 1.0 mg/kg) increased the number of VS presentations earned, while lower and higher VAR doses (0.01 and 3.0 mg/kg) did not change responding for the VS. VAR dose dependently attenuated the reinforcement-enhancing effects of NIC, with the highest dose (1.0 mg/kg) exhibiting the greatest antagonist effect. CONCLUSIONS: The results of these studies support the assertion that the therapeutic efficacy of VAR may be due to the partial agonist characteristics of the drug, specifically, its ability to partially replace the reinforcement-enhancing effects of NIC as well as antagonize these effects.


Subject(s)
Benzazepines/administration & dosage , Nicotine/antagonists & inhibitors , Nicotinic Agonists/administration & dosage , Quinoxalines/administration & dosage , Smoking Cessation/methods , Tobacco Use Disorder/drug therapy , Animals , Benzazepines/therapeutic use , Dose-Response Relationship, Drug , Male , Nicotinic Agonists/therapeutic use , Quinoxalines/therapeutic use , Rats , Rats, Sprague-Dawley , Reinforcement, Psychology , Varenicline
8.
Physiol Behav ; 104(1): 143-8, 2011 Jul 25.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21549139

ABSTRACT

Concerns about body weight represent an important barrier to public health efforts aimed at reducing smoking. Epidemiological studies have found that current smokers weigh less than non-smokers, smoking cessation results in weight gain, and weight restriction is commonly cited as a reason for smoking. The mechanisms underlying the relationship between smoking and weight are complex and may involve a number of factors including changes in caloric intake, physical activity, metabolic rate, and lipogenesis. Amongst these possible mechanisms, nicotine-induced enhancement of food reinforcement may be particularly important. In this paper, we first review data from our laboratory that highlight two distinct ways in which nicotine impacts reinforced behavior: 1) by acting as a primary reinforcer; and 2) by directly (non-associatively) enhancing the reinforcing effects of other stimuli. We then elaborate on the reinforcement-enhancing effects of nicotine as they pertain to behaviors and stimuli related to food. Data from both laboratory animals and humans support the assertion that nicotine enhances the reinforcing efficacy of food and suggest that the influence of these effects on eating may be most important after nicotine cessation when nicotine's effects on satiety subside. Finally, we discuss the theoretical and clinical implications of this perspective for understanding and addressing the apparent tradeoff between smoking and weight gain. Better understanding of the mechanisms underlying the reinforcement-enhancing effects of nicotine broadly, and the effects on food reinforcement per se, may aid in the development of new treatments with better long term outcomes.


Subject(s)
Body Weight/drug effects , Eating/drug effects , Nicotine/administration & dosage , Reinforcement, Psychology , Smoking , Animals , Feeding Behavior/drug effects , Humans
9.
Behav Pharmacol ; 21(7): 660-7, 2010 Oct.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20823771

ABSTRACT

Research conducted with rats has shown tolerance to the behavioral effects of psychomotor stimulants to be contingent on chronic drug administration occurring before the experimental session. Recent experiments with pigeons, however, resulted in tolerance when drug administration followed the experimental session. We hypothesized that the apparent species differences in tolerance may be a function of different operantly conditioned response topographies used in these experiments. Specifically, we propose that operantly reinforced consummatory responses, like pecking with pigeons, are less likely to reveal contingent tolerance. This experiment involved rats in a manner that paralleled earlier experiments with pigeons. Rats were subjected to daily sessions that required 20 licking responses to obtain 2.5-s access to water. Acute effects of cocaine were determined by administering precession doses ranging from 1.0 to 30.0 mg/kg, with dosing occurring every fifth day. Rats were then divided into two groups. One group received 17.0 mg/kg cocaine before the session and the other received 17.0 mg/kg cocaine after the session. After 30 daily sessions, the effects of the cocaine-dose range were reassessed. Tolerance to the rate-decreasing effects of cocaine was evident in both the groups, regardless of the temporal relationship between drug administration and the experimental session. The results support the hypothesis that operantly reinforced consummatory responses are less likely to show contingent tolerance.


Subject(s)
Cocaine , Drug Tolerance , Reinforcement Schedule , Animals , Behavioral Research , Cocaine/administration & dosage , Columbidae , Conditioning, Operant/drug effects , Dopamine Uptake Inhibitors/administration & dosage , Drug Tolerance/physiology , Models, Animal , Rats , Rats, Long-Evans
10.
J Exp Anal Behav ; 90(2): 207-18, 2008 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18831126

ABSTRACT

Tolerance to effects of cocaine can be modulated by schedules of reinforcement. With multiple ratio schedules, research has shown an inverse relationship between ratio requirement and amount of tolerance that resulted from daily administration of the drug. In contrast, tolerance to the effects of cocaine on behavior under multiple interval schedules generally has developed regardless of interval value. Under interval schedules reinforcement depends on the animal making one response following a time interval. Thus, as time to respond increases, the time to reinforcement decreases. On the other hand, fixed ratio schedules require a specified number of responses to be made prior to reinforcement. Therefore, delaying the initiation of responding does not coincide with a significant decrease in the time to reinforcement. In the current experiment, 6 pigeons were trained to respond under a three-component multiple schedule, with a different tandem fixed-ratio 1 fixed-interval schedule in each component. The multiple schedule required one response, which was followed by one of three fixed-interval values (5, 15, or 60 s). Thus, the multiple schedule was interval-like because after the fixed-ratio 1, only one more response was required for reinforcement, but it was also ratio-like because the length of the pause at the beginning of each interreinforcer interval affected the time until the next reinforcer. Acute administration of cocaine generally resulted in dose-dependent decreases in responding. Chronic (i.e., daily) administration of a rate-decreasing dose resulted in tolerance patterns similar to those usually obtained with multiple ratio schedules. That is, the magnitude of tolerance was related inversely to schedule size. These results suggest that delay to reinforcement from the initial response may play a role in the development of schedule-parameter-related tolerance.


Subject(s)
Behavior, Animal , Cocaine/administration & dosage , Self Administration , Animals , Columbidae , Female , Male
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...