Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 7 de 7
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
PLoS One ; 7(8): e42247, 2012.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22912692

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND: Insects have been among the most widely used model systems for studying the control of locomotion by nervous systems. In Drosophila, we implemented a simple test for locomotion: in Buridan's paradigm, flies walk back and forth between two inaccessible visual targets [1]. Until today, the lack of easily accessible tools for tracking the fly position and analyzing its trajectory has probably contributed to the slow acceptance of Buridan's paradigm. METHODOLOGY/PRINCIPAL FINDINGS: We present here a package of open source software designed to track a single animal walking in a homogenous environment (Buritrack) and to analyze its trajectory. The Centroid Trajectory Analysis (CeTrAn) software is coded in the open source statistics project R. It extracts eleven metrics and includes correlation analyses and a Principal Components Analysis (PCA). It was designed to be easily customized to personal requirements. In combination with inexpensive hardware, these tools can readily be used for teaching and research purposes. We demonstrate the capabilities of our package by measuring the locomotor behavior of adult Drosophila melanogaster (whose wings were clipped), either in the presence or in the absence of visual targets, and comparing the latter to different computer-generated data. The analysis of the trajectories confirms that flies are centrophobic and shows that inaccessible visual targets can alter the orientation of the flies without changing their overall patterns of activity. CONCLUSIONS/SIGNIFICANCE: Using computer generated data, the analysis software was tested, and chance values for some metrics (as well as chance value for their correlation) were set. Our results prompt the hypothesis that fixation behavior is observed only if negative phototaxis can overcome the propensity of the flies to avoid the center of the platform. Together with our companion paper, we provide new tools to promote Open Science as well as the collection and analysis of digital behavioral data.


Subject(s)
Drosophila melanogaster/physiology , Locomotion , Software , Animals , Behavior, Animal , Documentation , Female , Internet , Models, Theoretical , Principal Component Analysis , Stochastic Processes , Time Factors , Videotape Recording
2.
J Comput Neurosci ; 32(2): 197-212, 2012 Apr.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21698405

ABSTRACT

The pathways for olfactory learning in the fruitfly Drosophila have been extensively investigated, with mounting evidence that that the mushroom body is the site of the olfactory associative memory trace (Heisenberg, Nature 4:266-275, 2003; Gerber et al., Curr Opin Neurobiol 14:737-744, 2004). Heisenberg's description of the mushroom body as an associative learning device is a testable hypothesis that relates the mushroom body's function to its neural structure and input and output pathways. Here, we formalise a relatively complete computational model of the network interactions in the neural circuitry of the insect antennal lobe and mushroom body, to investigate their role in olfactory learning, and specifically, how this might support learning of complex (non-elemental; Giurfa, Curr Opin Neuroethol 13:726-735, 2003) discriminations involving compound stimuli. We find that the circuit is able to learn all tested non-elemental paradigms. This does not crucially depend on the number of Kenyon cells but rather on the connection strength of projection neurons to Kenyon cells, such that the Kenyon cells require a certain number of coincident inputs to fire. As a consequence, the encoding in the mushroom body resembles a unique cue or configural representation of compound stimuli (Pearce, Psychol Rev 101:587-607, 1994). Learning of some conditions, particularly negative patterning, is strongly affected by the assumption of normalisation effects occurring at the level of the antennal lobe. Surprisingly, the learning capacity of this circuit, which is a simplification of the actual circuitry in the fly, seems to be greater than the capacity expressed by the fly in shock-odour association experiments (Young et al. 2010).


Subject(s)
Learning/physiology , Models, Neurological , Olfactory Pathways/cytology , Olfactory Pathways/physiology , Sensory Receptor Cells/physiology , Animals , Arthropod Antennae/physiology , Computer Simulation , Drosophila , Mushroom Bodies/physiology , Odorants
3.
PLoS One ; 6(6): e20100, 2011.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21687789

ABSTRACT

Drosophila larvae change from exhibiting attraction to aversion as the concentration of salt in a substrate is increased. However, some aversive concentrations appear to act as positive reinforcers, increasing attraction to an odour with which they have been paired. We test whether this surprising dissociation between the unconditioned and conditioned response depends on the larvae's experience of salt concentration in their food. We find that although the point at which a NaCl concentration becomes aversive shifts with different rearing experience, the dissociation remains evident. Testing larvae using a substrate 0.025 M above the NaCl concentration on which the larvae were reared consistently results in aversive choice behaviour but appetitive reinforcement effects.


Subject(s)
Diet , Drosophila melanogaster/drug effects , Drosophila melanogaster/physiology , Food Preferences/drug effects , Learning/drug effects , Sodium Chloride/pharmacology , Adaptation, Physiological/drug effects , Animals , Dose-Response Relationship, Drug , Larva/drug effects , Larva/physiology , Reward
4.
Biol Cybern ; 99(2): 89-103, 2008 Aug.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18607623

ABSTRACT

The mushroom body is a prominent invertebrate neuropil strongly associated with learning and memory. We built a high-level computational model of this structure using simplified but realistic models of neurons and synapses, and developed a learning rule based on activity dependent pre-synaptic facilitation. We show that our model, which is consistent with mushroom body Drosophila data and incorporates Aplysia learning, is able to both acquire and later recall CS-US associations. We demonstrate that a highly divergent input connectivity to the mushroom body and strong periodic inhibition both serve to improve overall learning performance. We also examine the problem of how synaptic conductance, driven by successive training events, obtains a value appropriate for the stimulus being learnt. We employ two feedback mechanisms: one stabilises strength at an initial level appropriate for an association; another prevents strength increase for established associations.


Subject(s)
Association Learning/physiology , Computer Simulation , Conditioning, Classical/physiology , Drosophila , Models, Neurological , Action Potentials/physiology , Animals , Aplysia/anatomy & histology , Aplysia/physiology , Drosophila/anatomy & histology , Drosophila/physiology , Feedback, Physiological , Mushroom Bodies/cytology , Mushroom Bodies/physiology , Neurons/metabolism , Synapses/metabolism
5.
Proc Biol Sci ; 275(1637): 915-21, 2008 Apr 22.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18230590

ABSTRACT

Certain insect species are known to relocate nest or food sites using landmarks, but the generality of this capability among insects, and whether insect place memory can be used in novel task settings, is not known. We tested the ability of crickets to use surrounding visual cues to relocate an invisible target in an analogue of the Morris water maze, a standard paradigm for spatial memory tests on rodents. Adult female Gryllus bimaculatus were released into an arena with a floor heated to an aversive temperature, with one hidden cool spot. Over 10 trials, the time taken to find the cool spot decreased significantly. The best performance was obtained when a natural scene was provided on the arena walls. Animals can relocate the position from novel starting points. When the scene is rotated, they preferentially approach the fictive target position corresponding to the rotation. We note that this navigational capability does not necessarily imply the animal has an internal spatial representation.


Subject(s)
Gryllidae/physiology , Memory/physiology , Space Perception/physiology , Animals , Behavior, Animal/physiology , Vision, Ocular/physiology
6.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17180702

ABSTRACT

The resonant properties of the intrinsic dynamics of single neurons could play a direct role in behaviour. One plausible role is in the recognition of temporal patterns, such as that seen in the auditory communication systems of Orthoptera. Recent behavioural data from bushcrickets suggests that this behaviour has interesting resonance properties, but the underlying mechanism is unknown. Here we show that a very simple and general model for neural resonance could directly account for the different behavioural responses of bushcrickets to different song patterns.


Subject(s)
Animal Communication , Behavior, Animal/physiology , Gryllidae/physiology , Models, Neurological , Neurons/physiology , Pattern Recognition, Physiological/physiology , Acoustic Stimulation , Animals , Female , Gryllidae/cytology , Sound Spectrography
7.
Bioinspir Biomim ; 1(3): 63-75, 2006 Sep.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17671308

ABSTRACT

Although a variety of basic insect behaviours have inspired successful robot implementations, more complex capabilities in these 'simple' animals are often overlooked. By reviewing the general architecture of their nervous systems, we gain insight into how they are able to integrate behaviours, perform pattern recognition, context-dependent learning, and combine many sensory inputs in tasks such as navigation. We review in particular what is known about two specific 'higher' areas in the insect brain, the mushroom bodies and the central complex, and how they are involved in controlling an insect's behaviour. While much of the functional interpretation of this information is still speculative, it nevertheless suggests some promising new approaches to obtaining adaptive behaviour in robots.


Subject(s)
Behavior, Animal/physiology , Biomimetics/methods , Brain/physiology , Feedback/physiology , Insecta/physiology , Models, Neurological , Sensation/physiology , Animals , Robotics/methods
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...