Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 5 de 5
Filter
Add more filters










Database
Language
Publication year range
1.
Public Health Nutr ; 23(18): 3387-3393, 2020 12.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32912374

ABSTRACT

OBJECTIVE: To assess availability, variety, price and quality of different food products in a convenience sample of supermarkets in Germany and the USA. DESIGN: Cross-sectional study using an adapted version of the Bridging the Gap Food Store Observation Form. SETTING: Information on availability, quality, price and variety of selected food products in eight German and seven US supermarkets (discount and full service) was obtained and compared by country. RESULTS: A general tendency for lower prices of fruits and vegetables in Germany was observed, while produce quality and variety did not seem to differ between countries, with the exception of the variety of some vegetables such as tomatoes. Chips and cereals did not differ significantly in variety nor price. In both countries, high energy-dense foods were lower in energy costs than lower energy-dense foods. CONCLUSIONS: The influence of food prices and availability on consumption should be further explored, including the impact of country differences.


Subject(s)
Food Supply , Supermarkets , Beverages , Commerce , Cross-Sectional Studies , Food , Fruit , Germany , Humans , Vegetables
2.
Front Psychol ; 9: 1106, 2018.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30100887

ABSTRACT

Our ability to select relevant information from the environment is limited by the resolution of attention - i.e., the minimum size of the region that can be selected. Neural mechanisms that underlie this limit and its development are not yet understood. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) was performed during an object tracking task in 7- and 11-year-old children, and in young adults. Object tracking activated canonical fronto-parietal attention systems and motion-sensitive area MT in children as young as 7 years. Object tracking performance improved with age, together with stronger recruitment of parietal attention areas and a shift from low-level to higher-level visual areas. Increasing the required resolution of spatial attention - which was implemented by varying the distance between target and distractors in the object tracking task - led to activation increases in fronto-insular cortex, medial frontal cortex including anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and supplementary motor area, superior colliculi, and thalamus. This core circuitry for attentional precision was recruited by all age groups, but ACC showed an age-related activation reduction. Our results suggest that age-related improvements in selective visual attention and in the resolution of attention are characterized by an increased use of more functionally specialized brain regions during the course of development.

3.
PLoS One ; 9(6): e99298, 2014.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24926788

ABSTRACT

AIM: N-acyl dopamines (NADD) are gaining attention in the field of inflammatory and neurological disorders. Due to their hydrophobicity, NADD may have access to the endoplasmic reticulum (ER). We therefore investigated if NADD induce the unfolded protein response (UPR) and if this in turn influences cell behaviour. METHODS: Genome wide gene expression profiling, confirmatory qPCR and reporter assays were employed on human umbilical vein endothelial cells (HUVEC) to validate induction of UPR target genes and UPR sensor activation by N-octanoyl dopamine (NOD). Intracellular ATP, apoptosis and induction of thermotolerance were used as functional parameters to assess adaptation of HUVEC. RESULTS: NOD, but not dopamine dose dependently induces the UPR. This was also found for other synthetic NADD. Induction of the UPR was dependent on the redox activity of NADD and was not caused by selective activation of a particular UPR sensor. UPR induction did not result in cell apoptosis, yet NOD strongly impaired cell proliferation by attenuation of cells in the S-G2/M phase. Long-term treatment of HUVEC with low NOD concentration showed decreased intracellular ATP concentration paralleled with activation of AMPK. These cells were significantly more resistant to cold inflicted injury. CONCLUSIONS: We provide for the first time evidence that NADD induce the UPR in vitro. It remains to be assessed if UPR induction is causally associated with hypometabolism and thermotolerance. Further pharmacokinetic studies are warranted to address if the NADD concentrations used in vitro can be obtained in vivo and if this in turn shows therapeutic efficacy.


Subject(s)
Adenosine Triphosphate/metabolism , Dopamine/analogs & derivatives , Endothelial Cells/drug effects , Unfolded Protein Response/drug effects , AMP-Activated Protein Kinases/genetics , AMP-Activated Protein Kinases/metabolism , Apoptosis , Cell Proliferation/drug effects , Cell Survival/drug effects , Dopamine/pharmacology , Endothelial Cells/metabolism , Gene Expression Profiling , Gene Expression Regulation/drug effects , Human Umbilical Vein Endothelial Cells , Humans , In Vitro Techniques
4.
J Ind Microbiol Biotechnol ; 39(3): 503-11, 2012 Mar.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22042517

ABSTRACT

Modular polyketide synthases (PKSs) from Streptomyces and related genera of bacteria produce many important pharmaceuticals. A program called CompGen was developed to carry out in silico homologous recombination between gene clusters encoding PKSs and determine whether recombinants have cluster architectures compatible with the production of polyketides. The chemical structure of recombinant polyketides was also predicted. In silico recombination was carried out for 47 well-characterised clusters. The predicted recombinants would produce 11,796 different polyketide structures. The molecular weights and average degree of reduction of the chemical structures are dispersed around the parental structures indicating that they are likely to include pharmaceutically interesting compounds. The details of the recombinants and the chemical structures were entered in a database called r-CSDB. The virtual compound library is a useful resource for computer-aided drug design and chemoinformatics strategies for finding pharmaceutically relevant chemical entities. A strategy to construct recombinant Streptomyces strains to produce these polyketides is described and the critical steps of mobilizing large biosynthetic clusters and producing new linear cloning vectors are illustrated by experimental data.


Subject(s)
Polyketide Synthases/metabolism , Streptomyces/metabolism , Amino Acid Sequence , Bacterial Proteins/chemistry , Bacterial Proteins/metabolism , Bioengineering , Homologous Recombination , Models, Molecular , Multigene Family , Polyketide Synthases/chemistry , Polyketide Synthases/genetics , Recombinant Proteins/chemistry , Recombinant Proteins/metabolism , Software , Streptomyces/genetics
5.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 71(1): 26-41, 2009 Jan.
Article in English | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19304594

ABSTRACT

In two experiments, the effects of implicit attentional learning and habituation on inhibition of return (IOR) were assessed. Experiment 1 consisted of an informative variant of the spatial cuing paradigm (Posner, 1980), with the target appearing most often at the location next to the cue. Short-term as well as long-term learning of the regularities could be observed. This learning took place even though the participants were not able to verbalize the learned information, suggesting that the learning was implicit. By spatially separating the effects of implicit learning and IOR, we demonstrated that the magnitude of IOR slightly decreased because of long-term implicit orienting. In Experiment 2, a noninformative variant of the spatial cuing paradigm, IOR was still found to decrease over the course of the experiment. This suggests that habituation due to the mere repeated initiation of IOR also affects the magnitude of the IOR. This theory is supported by the finding that IOR recovered after short breaks in Experiment 2. However, habituation presumably only played a secondary role in Experiment 1. In sum, IOR was abolished by neither implicit attentional learning nor habituation. Here, the finding that IOR is not easily replaced by implicitly learned orienting clearly indicates that IOR is a powerful, low-level mechanism. We discuss implications of these results for the debate on the function of IOR.


Subject(s)
Attention , Cues , Habituation, Psychophysiologic , Inhibition, Psychological , Orientation , Pattern Recognition, Visual , Reaction Time , Adult , Association Learning , Female , Fixation, Ocular , Humans , Male , Set, Psychology , Young Adult
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL
...